Skip to main content
Log in

Use of current situational information and causal inference: Do dysphoric individuals make “unwarranted” causal inferences?

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

According to Beck's original theory, depressives make unwarranted negatively biased personal inferences. Specifically, Beck suggested that depressives ignore current positive situational information and are unduly influenced by current negative situational information in making inferences. To test Beck's theory, we used Kelley's normative model of causal inference to examine the utilization of causally relevant situational information by dysphoric, nondepressed, and very nondepressed subjects in making causal attributions for personal success and failure. We used Stevens and Jones' classic method from social psychology and embedded the relevant causal information in the natural flow of events. Results showed that dysphoric, nondepressed, and very nondepressed subjects did, to an equal degree, use such information to make causal attributions. Although dysphoric and both groups of nondepressed subjects used current situational information consistently with Kelley's model, clear-cut baseline differences in the content of their causal attributions existed. Thus, the results supported the reformulations of Beck's theory that emphasize content, rather than process, differences between depressive and nondepressive cognition for dysphoria.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression.Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman, R., & DeRubeis, R. (1991). Is depressive realism real?Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahrens, A. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (1991). Changes in personal standards and dysphoria: A Longitudinal approach.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 47–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., Albright, J. S., Abramson, L. Y., & Dykman, B. M. (1990). Depressive realism and nondepressive optimistic illusions: The role of the self. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.),Contemporary psychological approaches to depression: Treatment, research, and theory. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Tabachnik, N. (1984). Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information.Psychological Review, 91, 112–149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1987). Cognitive models of depression.Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1, 5–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1991). Cognitive therapy: A 30-year retrospective.American Psychologist, 46, 368–375.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests.Psychometrika, 16, 297–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dykman, B. M., & Abramson, L. Y. (1990). Contributions of basic research to the cognitive theories of depression.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 42–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dykman, B. M., Abramson, L. Y., & Albright, J. (1991). Effects of ascending and descending patterns of success and failure upon dysphoric and nondysphoric subjects: Encoding, recall, and predictions of future success.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 179–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dykman, B. M., Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Hartlage, S. (1989). Processing of ambiguous and unambiguous feedback by depressed and nondepressed college students: Schematic biases and their implications for depressive realism.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 431–445.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dykman, B. M., Horowitz, L. M., Abramson, L. Y., & Usher, M. (1991). Schematic and situational determinants of depressed and nondepressed students' interpretation of feedback.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 45–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, J. A., & Haaga, D. A. F. (1994).Cognitive bias and irrational beliefs in major depression and dysphoria. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Haack, L. J., Metalsky, G. I., Dykman, B. M., & Abramson, L. Y. (1990).Disambiguating causal dilemmas: A comparison of dysphoric and nondysphoric subjects. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wisconsin, Madison.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haaga, D. A. F., & Beck, A. T. (1995). Perspectives on depressive realism: Implications for cognitive theory of depression.Behavior Research and Therapy, 33, 41–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haaga, D. A. F., Dyck, M. J., & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory of depression.Psychological Bulletin, 110, 215–236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haaga, D. A. F., & Solomon, A. (1993). Impact of Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hammen, and Ingram (1987) on treatment of the continuity issue in “depression” research.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 313–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression.Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., & Garber, J. (1988). Cognitive therapy. In L. Y. Abramson (Ed.),Social cognition and clinical psychology: A synthesis (pp. 204–253). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horwath, E., Johnson, J., Klerman, G. L., & Weissman, M. M. (1992). Depressive symptoms as relative and attributable risk factors for first-onset major depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 817–823.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, L. A., & Larrance, D. T. (1979). Is a “refinement” of attribution theory necessary to accommodate the learned helplessness reformulation? A critique of the reformulation of Abramson, Seligman, and Teasdale.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 681–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, K. L., & Metalsky, G. I. (1994). Low-end specificity of the Beck Depression Inventory.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 55–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution.American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., & Flannery-Schroeder, E. C. (1995). Rigor, but not rigor mortis, in depression research.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 892–894.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klerman, G. L., & Weissman, M. M. (1992). The course, morbidity, and costs of depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 831–834.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1977). Empirical-clinical approach toward a definition of childhood depression. In J. G. Schulterbrandt & A. Raskin (Eds.),Depression in childhood: Diagnosis, treatment, and conceptual models. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, S. E., & Liu, C. (1987). The effect of mood and information valence on depressive cognition.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 185–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 171–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metalsky, G. I., & Abramson, L. Y. (1981). Attributional styles: Toward a framework for conceptualization and assessment. In P. C. Kendall & S. D. Hollon (Eds.),Assessment strategies for cognitive-behavioral interventions. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction?Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., & Ross, L. (1980).Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self perception and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880–892.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1985). Attribution and social perception. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.),The handbook of social psychology: Vol. II (pp. 73–122). New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, L., & Jones, E. E. (1976). Defensive attribution and the Kelley cube.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 809–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional style in depression: A meta-analytic review.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 974–991.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion.Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wortman, C. B., Costanzo, P. R., & Witt, T. R. (1973). Effect of anticipated performance on the attributions of causality to self and other.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 372–381.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Preparation of this article was supported by a Vilas Award, a University of Wisconsin Graduate School Grant, a Romnes Fellowship, a Biomedical Grant, and NIMH Grant R01MH43866 to Lyn Y. Abramson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Haack, L.J., Metalsky, G.I., Dykman, B.M. et al. Use of current situational information and causal inference: Do dysphoric individuals make “unwarranted” causal inferences?. Cogn Ther Res 20, 309–331 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02228036

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02228036

Key words

Navigation