Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of two levels of procedural fidelity with constant time delay on children's learning

  • Regular Papers
  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of constant time delay delivered with high procedural fidelity to constant time delay with high procedural fidelity on all variables except delivery of the controlling prompt (i.e., on a mean of 44% of the trials, the controlling prompt was not delivered when it should have been provided). Six preschool children with disabilities were taught to identify photographs in two alternating conditions (e.g., high procedural fidelity and low procedural fidelity). An adapted alternating treatments design was used to evaluate the instructional conditions on the effectiveness and efficiency of instruction. In addition, daily measures were taken of the teacher's implementation of each step of the constant time delay procedures which indicated that the two conditions were implemented as planned. The results indicate that both conditions were effective for four children; for three of these, the high procedural fidelity condition resulted in more efficient learning. For the fifth child, the high-fidelity condition resulted in criterion level responding, but the low fidelity condition did not. However, when the high fidelity procedure and trial-by-trial reinforcement were used for the low-fidelity stimuli, these also were acquired. For the sixth child, neither procedure was effective; thus, the high fidelity condition was used alone and resulted in learning. The results are discussed in terms of using the constant time delay procedure and studying the procedural fidelity of other strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ault, M. J., Gast, D. L., & Wolery, M. (1988). Comparison of progressive and constant time delay procedures in teaching community-sign word reading.American Journal of Mental Retardation, 93, 44–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ault, M. J., Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., Doyle, P. M., & Eizenstat, V. (1988). Comparison of response prompting procedures in teaching numeral identification to autistic subjects.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 18, 627–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Billingsley, F. F., White, O. R., & Munson, R. (1980). Procedural reliability: A rationale and an example.Behavioral Assessment, 2, 229–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley-Johnson, S., Sunderman, P., & Johnson, C. M. (1983). Comparison of delayed prompting and fading for teaching preschoolers easily confused letters and numbers.Journal of School Psychology, 21, 327–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browder, P. M., Morris, W. W., & Snell, M. E. (1981). Using time delay to teach manual signs to a severely retarded student.Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 16, 252–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gast, D. L., Ault, M. J., Wolery, M., Doyle, P. M., & Belanger, S. (1988). Comparison of constant time delay and the system of least prompts in teaching sight word reading to students with moderate retardation.Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 23, 117–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handen, B. L., & Zane, T. (1987). Delayed prompting: A review of procedural variations and results.Research in Developmental Disabilities, 8, 307–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, J. Y. K. (1979).Photo cue cards. Tucson, AZ: Communication Skill Builders, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinert, H. L., & Gast, D. L. (1982). Teaching a multihandicapped adult manual signs using a constant time delay procedure.Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped, 6(4), 25–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeLaurin, K., & Wolery, M. (1992). Research standards in early intervention: Defining, describing, and measuring the independent variable.Journal of Early Intervention, 16, 275–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattingly, J. C., & Bott, D. A. (1990). Use of a constant time delay procedure in teaching multiplication facts to mildly handicapped students.Exceptional Children, 56, 438–449.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDonnell, J. (1987). The effects of time delay and increasing prompt hierarchy strategies on the acquisition of purchasing skills by students with severe handicaps.Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped, 12, 227–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonnell, J., & Ferguson, B. (1989). A comparison of time delay and decreasing prompt hierarchy strategies in teaching banking skills to students with moderate handicaps.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 85–91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, M. A., Shores, R. E., Wehby, J. H., Johnson, S. M., & Fox, J. J. (1990). Special education teachers' implementation procedures to promote social interaction among children in integrated settings.Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 25, 267–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, U. C., & Test, D. W. (1989). A comparison of constant time delay and most-to-least prompting in teaching laundry skills to students with moderate retardation.Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 24, 363–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newborg, J., Stock, J., Wnek, L., Guidubaldi, J., & Svinicki J. (1984).Battelle Developmental Inventory. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. A., & McConnell, S. R. (1993). Factors affecting the impact of social interaction skills interventions in early childhood special education.Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 13, 38–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoen, S. F., & Sivil, E. O. (1989). A comparison of procedures in teaching self-help skills: Increasing assistance, time delay, and observational learning.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 57–72.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuster, J. W., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., & Guiltinan, S. (1988). The effectiveness of a constant time delay procedure to teach chained responses to adolescents with mental retardation.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 169–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuster, J. W., Griffen, A. K., & Wolery, M. (1992). A comparison of constant time delay procedure and simultaneous prompting in teaching food words to elementary-aged students with moderate mental retardation.Journal of Behavioral Education, 2, 305–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sindelar, P. T., Rosenberg, M. S., & Wilson, R. J. (1985). An adapted alternating treatments design for instructional research.Education and Treatment of Children, 8(1), 67–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, K. B., Blackhurst, A. E., & Slaton, D. B. (1991). Teaching memorized spellings with a microcomputer: Time delay and computer assisted instruction.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 153–160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Touchette, P. E. (1971). Transfer of stimulus control: Measuring the moment of transfer.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 347–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilbers, J. S., & Wolery, M. (1991, May).Effects of high and low procedural fidelity during the response interval of constant time delay. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Atlanta, GA.

  • Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Doyle, P. M. (1992).Teaching students with moderate and severe disabilities: Use of response prompting strategies. White Plains, NY: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., Gast, D. L., Doyle, P. M., & Griffen, A. K. (1990). Comparison of constant time delay and the system of least prompts in teaching chained tasks.Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 25, 243–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., Gast, D. L., Doyle, P. M., & Mills, B. M. (1990). Use of choral and individual attentional responses with constant time delay when teaching sight word reading.Remedial and Special Education, 11, 47–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolery, M., Holcombe, A., Cybriwsky, C., Doyle, P. M., Schuster, J. W., Ault, M. J., Gast, D. L. (1992). Constant time delay with discrete responses: A review of effectiveness and demographic, procedural, and methodological parameters.Research in Developmental Disabilities, 13, 239–266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1979).Preschool Language Scale. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Holcombe, A., Wolery, M. & Snyder, E. Effects of two levels of procedural fidelity with constant time delay on children's learning. J Behav Educ 4, 49–73 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01560509

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01560509

Key words

Navigation