Skip to main content
Log in

Gender stereotypes are not accurate: A replication of Martin (1987) using diagnostic vs. self-report and behavioral criteria

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Women and men (58 females and 42 males) reported whether they possess each of several stereotypic and neutral traits (self-report criterion) and recorded the percentage of each gender they believed display each trait (diagnostic). Observations of subjects (57 males and 49 females) discussing controversial topics yielded the behavioral criterion. (Approximately 81% of the total sample was European-American, 8% African-American, 2% Hispanic, 1% Asian, and the remainder of “other” ethnicity.) Gender differences were larger and more inaccurate in stereotypes (diagnostic) than in “reality” (self-report criterion), but behavioral criterion results were mixed. Difficulties in specifying criteria for “actual differences,” including behavioral criteria, and recent work related to C. L. Martin's [(1987) “A Ratio Measure of Gender Stereotyping,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 52, pp. 489–499] paradigm (and results) are considered. Implications of results for the importance of replication and for research on accuracy of stereotypes are also discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, B. P. (1994).Theories of personality. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, B. P., & Potkay, C. R. (1983).Adjective Generation Technique (AGT): Research and applications. New York: Irvington Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. A., & Byrne, D. (1994).Social psychology (7th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1981).The Bem Sex-Role Inventory: A professional manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M., & Manis, M. (1994). Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgments.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 5–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, G. V. (1988). Stereotypic biases in social decision making and memory: Testing process models of stereotype use.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 726–737.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brigham, J. C. (1971). Ethnic stereotypes.Psychological Bulletin, 76, 15–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brigham, J. C. (1986).Social psychology. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987).Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. (1981). Sex of researchers and sex-typed communications as determinants of sex differences in influence ability: A meta-analysis of social influence studies.Psychological Bulletin, 90, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Steffan, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 735–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenberg, L. (1990). Improving the accuracy of stereotypes within the workplace.Journal of Management, 16, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, C., & Hurst, N. (1990). Gender stereotypes: Perception or rationalization?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 197–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1993). Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes.Psychological Review, 10, 109–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jussim, L. (1991). Social perception and social reality: A reflection-construction model.Psychological Review, 98, 54–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1987). A ratio measure of gender stereotyping.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 489–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, C., & Stitt, C. L. (1978). An individual and quantitative measure of stereotypes.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 929–940.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality.Psychological Review, 80, 252–283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results.Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1673–1682.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effects sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 21–36.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Thanks are due Nancy Hobby and Brett Hart for conducting part of the study. Special thanks are also due Carol Martin for providing numerous thoughtful and helpful suggestions.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Allen, B.P. Gender stereotypes are not accurate: A replication of Martin (1987) using diagnostic vs. self-report and behavioral criteria. Sex Roles 32, 583–600 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01544213

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01544213

Keywords

Navigation