Skip to main content
Log in

Intellectual ability, rationality, and intuitiveness as predictors of warranted and unwarranted optimism for future life events

  • Published:
Journal of Youth and Adolescence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The social calculations involved in assessing one's life chances were examined. Undergraduates estimated the probability that they would experience positive and negative life events and the probability that others would experience the same events. Participants evinced considerable bias in their estimates, expressing the belief that they would experience more desirable and fewer undesirable events than others. Important individual differences in these biases were found. Particularly regarding academic/career events, those with superior intellectual ability expressed more bias than their peers. Surprisingly, even students in the bottom third of intellectual ability displayed considerable academic/career bias. Cognitive style was also linked with the extensiveness of biased perceptions: individuals high in Need for Cognition were more optimistically biased than their peers in the academic/career domain. Also, relative to participants whose cognitive style was more rational, those who relied on intuitive processing believed that both positive and negative events were more likely to be experienced, by both the self and others. Finally, students who eventually dropped out of college were lower in Need for Cognition and had more unrealistic expectations regarding both positive and negative life events than students who remained in school. These findings are discussed in terms of general adolescent phenomena, such as individuation, identity, and the personal fable, and cognitive factors, such as the knowledge of base rate information and the availability heuristic.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alloy, L. B., and Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for self and others.J. Personal. Social Psychol. 52: 366–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnett, J. (1992). Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective.Develop. Rev. 12: 339–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blieszner, Willis, S. L., and Baltes, P. B. (1981). Training research in aging on the fluid ability of inductive reasoning.J. App. Develop. Psychol. 2: 247–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blos, P. (1967). The second individuation process of adolescence.Psychoanal. Study Child 22: 162–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., and Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition.J. Personal. Social Psychol. 42: 116–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., and Story, A. L. (1991). Depression, realism, and the overconfidence effect: Are the sadder wiser when predicting future actions and events.J. Personal. Social Psychol. 61: 521–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence.Child Develop. 38: 1025–1034.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In Pervin, L. (ed.),Handbook and Personality Theory and Research. Guilford, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, and Heier, H. (1994). Individual differences in intuitive and analytical information processing. Unpublished manuscript: University of Massachusetts.

  • Erikson, E. (1968).Identity: Youth and Crisis. Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillies, P., Elmwood, J. M., and Hawtin, P. (1985). Anxieties in adolescents about employment and war.Br. Med. J. 291: 383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havighurst, R. J. (1972).Developmental Tasks and Education (3rd ed.). McKay, New York. (Original work published in 1948.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Klaczynski, P. A., and Reese, H. W. (1991). Educational trajectory and “action orientation”: grade and track differences.J. Youth Adolesc. 20: 441–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamm, H., Schmidt, R. W., and Trommsdorf, G. (1976). Sex and social class as determinants of future orientation (time perspective).J. Personal. Social Psychol. 34: 317–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lapsky, D. K., Milstead, M., Quintana, S. M., Flannery, D., and Buss, R. R. (1986). Adolescent egocentrism and formal operations: Tests of a theoretical assumption.Develop. Psychol. 22: 800–807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lens, W., and Gailly, A. (1980). Extension of future time perspective in motivational goals of three different age groups.Human Develop. 32: 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J. W. (1977). The effects of education as an institution.Am. J. Sociol. 83: 55–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neugarten, B. L., and Hagestad, G. O. (1976). Age and life course. In Binstock, R. H., and Shanas, E. (eds.),Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., and Ross, L. (1980).Human Inference. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nurmi, J. E. (1989). Development of orientation to the future during early adolescence: A four-year longitudinal study and two cross-sectional comparisons.Int. J. Psychol. 24: 195–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nurmi, J. E. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning.Develop. Rev. 11: 1–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nurmi, J. E. (1992). Age differences in adult life goals, concerns, and their temporal extension: A life course approach to future-oriented motivation.Int. J. Behav. Develop. 15: 487–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nurmi, J. E., and Pullianen, H. (1991). The changing parent-child relationship, self-esteem, and intelligence as determinants of orientation to the future during adolescence.J. Adolesc. 14: 35–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, M. A. (1988). Adolescent fears: Some Caribbean findings.J. Youth Adolesc. 17: 255–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., and Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others.J. Personal. Social Psychol. 52: 994–1001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., and Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health.Psychol. Bull. 103: 193–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, T. G. (1962).Primary Mental Abilities for Grades 9-12. Science Research Associates, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verstraeten, D. (1980). Level of realism in adolescent future time perspective.Human Develop. 23: 177–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life event.J. Personal. Social Psychol 39: 806–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, S. L., and Schaie, K. W. (1986). Practical intelligence in later adulthood. In Sternberg, R. J., and Wagner, R. K. (eds.),Practical Intelligence: Nature and Origns of Competence in the Everyday World. Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Received Ph.D. in 1989 from West Virginia University.

Research interests include contextual influences on adolescence reasoning and problem solving, the impact of motivational and context-sensitive goals on reasoning and problem solving, and the interactions among adolescent critical thinking, self-serving biases, and identity information.

Received M. A. in Clinical Psychology from Western Carolina University. Currently enrolled in Penn State University Ph.D. Program in Counseling Psychology. Research interests include intellectual development and assessment, personality assessment, and work place adjustment.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Klaczynski, P.A., Fauth, J.M. Intellectual ability, rationality, and intuitiveness as predictors of warranted and unwarranted optimism for future life events. J Youth Adolescence 25, 755–773 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537452

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537452

Keywords

Navigation