Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive theories of vulnerability to depression: Reconciling negative evidence

Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive theories of depression propose that stable beliefs predispose vulnerable individuals to depression. Empirical evidence appears to contradict the stability hypothesis; the cognitions described by the theories appear to covary with depressive symptoms. As a result of these findings, many investigators have concluded that the etiological portions of the cognitive theories are incorrect. We propose an alternative account of the empirical evidence that is consistent with the theories. We propose that the beliefs that are vulnerability factors for depression are stable, but they are accessible only during negative mood states; we call this the mood-state hypothesis. This article describes the mood-state hypothesis, reviews evidence supporting it, and outlines its implications for the cognitive theories of depression. We also describe implications of the mood-state hypothesis for other theories of depression, including biological theories, for studies of psychotherapy process, for studies of other psychopathologies, for epidemiological studies of depression, for the prevention and treatment of depression, and for theories of cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression.Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arieti, S., & Bemporad, J. R. (1980). The psychological organization of depression.American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1360–1365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Psychosocial functioning and depression. Distinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences.Psychological Bulletin, 104, 97–126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In P J. Clayton & J. E. Barrett (Eds.),Treatment of depression: Old controversies and new approaches. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985).Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackburn, I. M., Eunson, K. M., & Bishop, S. (1986). A two-year naturalistic follow-up of depressed patients treated with cognitive therapy, pharmacotherapy and a combination of both.Journal of Affective Disorders, 10, 67–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blackburn, I. M., & Smyth, P. (1985). A test of cognitive vulnerability in individuals prone to depression.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 61–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review.Psychological Bulletin, 99 229–246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective depression.Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 29, 106–1604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1986).Temporary emotional states act like multiple personalities. Speech delivered at the Conference on Multiple Personality and Dissociative Disorders, September 18–20, 1986.

  • Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory.Behavior Research and Therapy, 25, 443–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewin, C. R. (1985). Depression and causal attributions: What is their relation?Psychological Bulletin, 98, 297–309.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chadoff, P. (1970). The depressive personality: A critical review.Archives of General Psychiatry, 73, 666–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression: A critical appraisal.Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472–505.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R. A., & Monroe, S. M. (1983). Psychopathology research. In M. Hersen, A. E. Kazdin, & A. S. Bellack (Eds.),The clinical psychology handbook (pp. 239–264). New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobson, K. S., & Shaw, B. F. (1986). Cognitive assessment with major depressive disorders.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 13–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobson, K. S., & Shaw, B. F. (1987). Specificity and stability of self-referent encoding in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 34–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohr, K. V., Rush, A. J., & Bernstein, I. H. (1989). Cognitive biases and depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 263–267.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, G., & Rush, A. J. (1984). Cognitive patterns in symptomatic and remitted unipolar major depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fennell, M. J. V., & Campbell, E. A. (1984). The Cognition Questionnaire: Specific thinking errors in depression.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 81–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information.Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, E. W., & Abramson, L. Y. (1983). Cognitive patterns and major depressive disorder: A longitudinal study in a hospital setting.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Ellicott, A., Gitlin, M., & Jamison, K. R. (1989). Sociotropy/autonomy and vulnerability to specific life events in patients with unipolar depression and bipolar disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 154–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Marks, T., deMayo, R., & Mayol, A. (1985a). Self-schemas and risk for depression: A prospective study.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49 1147–1159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Marks, T., Mayol, A., & deMayo, R. (1985b). Depressive self-schemas, life stress, and vulnerability to depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 308–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Miklowitz, D., & Dyck, D. (1986). Stability and severity parameters of depressive self-schema responding.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 23–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L. Chodoff, P., Korchin, S., & Barrett, J. (1976). Dependency—self-esteem—clinical depression.Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 4, 373–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognition in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 52–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 443–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, D. M., Harding, K., Taylor, E. B., & Dickstein, S. (1988). Dependency and self-criticism in depression: Evaluation in a clinical population.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 399–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1977). Imagery in therapy: An information processing analysis of fear.Behavior Therapy, 8, 862–886.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J. L., Larson, D. W., & Franklin, J. (1981). Depression related cognition: Antecedents or consequence?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 213–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlatt, G. A. (1985). Relapse prevention: Theoretical rationale and overview of the model. In G. A. Marlatt & J. R. Gordon (Eds.),Relapse prevention (pp. 3–70). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millon, T., & Kotik, D. (1985). The relationship of deprsson to disorders of personality. In E. E. Beckham & W. R. Leber,Handbook of depression: Treatment, assessment, and research. Homewood, Illinois: The Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, J., & Persons, J. B. (1988). Dysfunctional attitudes are mood-state dependent.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 76–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, J., Persons, J. B., & Byers, C. N. (1990). Endorsement of dysfunctional beliefs depends on current mood state.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99 237–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muñoz, R. F., Glish, M., Soo-Hoo, T., & Robertson, J. (1982). The San Francisco Mood Survey Project: Preliminary work toward the prevention of depression.American Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 317–329.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Persons, J. B., & Miranda, J. (1991). Treating dysfunctional beliefs: Implications of the mood-state hypothesis.Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly.5, 15–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Persons, J. B., & Rao, P. A. (1985). Longitudinal study of cognition, life events, and depression in psychiatric inpatients.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 51–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reda, M. A., Carpiniello, B., Secchiaroli, L., & Blanco, S. Y. (1985). Thinking, depression, and antidepressants: Modified and unmodified depressive beliefs during treatment with amitriptyline.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 135–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rehm, L. P., Kaslow, N. J., & Rabin, A. S. (1987). Cogntivie and behavioral targets in a self-control therapy program for depression.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 60–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H. (1989). The medicating mechanisms in mood and memory: A cognitive-priming formulation. In D. Kuiken (Ed.), Mood and memory: Theory, research, and applications (Special Issue).Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Rholes, W. S. (1984). Cognitive accessibility and the capacity of cognition to predict future depression: A theoretical note.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosse, R. B., Owen, C. M., & Horihisa, J. M. (1987). Brain imaging and laboratory testing in neuropsychiatry. In R. E. Hales & S. C. Yudofsky (Eds.),Textbook of Neuropsychiatry (Chap. 2, pp. 17–38). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rush, A. J. Beck, A. T., Kovacs, M., Weissenburger, J., & Hollon, S. D. (1982). Comparison of the effects of cognitive therapy on hopelessness and self-concept.American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 862–866.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression.Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147–162.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., & Shaw, B. F. (1986). Cognition in depression: A reappraisal of Coyne and Gotlib's critique.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 671–693.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., Castellon, C., Cacciola, J., Schulman, P., Luborsky, L., Ollove, M., & Downing, R. (1988). Explanatory style change during cognitive therapy for unipolar depression.Journal of Abnormal psychology, 97, 13–18.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, J. S., Silverman, J. A., & Eardley, D. A. (1984a). Do maladaptive attitudes cause depression?Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 28–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, J. S., Silverman, J. A., & Eardley, D. A. (1984b).Do maladaptive attitudes cause depression: Misconceptions of cognitive theory.Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 1112.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, A. D., Garfield, S. L., & Murphy, G. E. (1984). The process of change in cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression.Archives of General psychiatry, 41, 45–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression.Cognition and Emotion, 2 247–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Dent, J. (1987). Cognitive vulnerability to depression: An investigation of two hypothesis.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 113–126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for the induction of mood states.Behavior Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, A. (1979). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: A validation study.Dissertation Abstracts International, 40, 1389–1390B. (University Microfilm No. 79-19, 533)

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, A., & Beck, A. (1978, November).Development and validation of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Chicago.

  • Wilkinson, I. M., & Blackburn, I. M. (1981). Cognitive style in depressed and recovered depressed patients.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 283–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeiss, A. M., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Muñoz, R. F. (1979). Nonspecific improvement effects in depression using interpersonal skills training, pleasant activity schedules, or cognitive training.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 427–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We thank Aaron T. Beck for helpful comments on an earlier version of this article and John Rosenberg for his assistance with the biological section. Portions of this paper were presented at the meetings of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, June 26–30, 1990, Wintergreen, Virginia.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Persons, J.B., Miranda, J. Cognitive theories of vulnerability to depression: Reconciling negative evidence. Cogn Ther Res 16, 485–502 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01183170

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01183170

Key words

Navigation