Skip to main content
Log in

Attentional processing in clinically depressed subjects: A longitudinal investigation

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study was designed to examine attentional biases in information processing in depressed subjects both during periods of depression and following recovery. On two occasions, approximately 3 months apart, clinically depressed and nondepressed subjects completed a focused-attention dichotic listening task and a concurrent light-probe reaction-time task. As predicted, whereas depressed subjects took longer to respond to light probes when negative-content words were presented in the unattended channel of the dichotic tape than they did when either positive- or neutral-content words were presented, nondepressed subjects' reaction times did not differ across the three conditions. Results from the second session indicated that recovered subjects no longer demonstrated attentional biases. Specifically, the performance of recovered depressed subjects was less disrupted by the negative-content stimuli. These findings suggest that accessibility to negative stimuli is facilitated only during periods of depression, and that such accessibility is unlikely to provide the method whereby cognitive schemata exert a vulnerability for future depressions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bargh, J. A. (1982). Attention and automaticity in the processing of self-relevant information.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43 425–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic processing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 925–939.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Psychosocial functioning and depression: Distinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences.Psychological Bulletin, 104 97–126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Beamesderfer, A. (1974). Assessment of depression: The depression inventory. In P. Pichot (Ed.),Psychological measurement in psychopharmacology: Modern problems in pharmacopsychiatry (Vol. 7, pp. 151–169). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 4 561–571.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belsher, G., & Costello, C. G. (1988). Relapse after recovery from unipolar depression: A critical review.Psychological Bulletin, 104 84–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36 129–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20 37–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression: A critical appraisal.Psychological Bulletin, 94 472–505.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1986). Studying the role of cognitions in depression: Well-trodden paths and cul-de-sacs.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10 695–705.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobson, K. S., & Shaw, B. F. (1987). Specificity and stability of self-referent encoding in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96 34–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, G., & Rush, A. J. (1984). Cognitive patterns in symptomatic and remitted unipolar major depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia.Archives of General Psychiatry, 35 837–844.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Depressive disorders. In A. S. Bellack & M. Hersen (Eds.),Psychopathology in adulthood: An advanced text (pp. 179–194). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Cane, D. B. (1987). Construct accessibility and clinical depression: A longitudinal investigation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96 199–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Cane, D. B. (1989). Self-report assessment of depression and anxiety. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson, (Eds.),Anxiety and Depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 131–169). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & McCabe, S. B. (1992). An information-processing approach to the study of cognitive functioning in depression. In E. F. Walker, B. A. Cornblatt, & R. H. Dworkin (Eds.),Progress in experimental personality and psychopathology research (Vol. 15, pp. 131–161). New York: Springer Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & McCann, C. D. (1984). Construct accessibility and depression: An examination of cognitive and affective factors.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47 427–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., McLachlan, A. L., & Katz, A. N. (1988). Biases in visual attentional in depressed and nondepressed individuals.Cognition and Emotion, 2 185–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Meyer, J. P. (1986). Factor analysis of the Multiple Affect Adjective Check List: A separation of positive and negative affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50 1161–1165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Whiffen, V. E. (1989). Stress, coping, and marital satisfaction in couples with a depressed wife.Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21 401–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., Whiffen, V. E., Mount, J. H., Milne, K., & Cordy, N. I. (1989). Prevalence rates and demographic characteristics associated with depression in pregnancy and the postpartum.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57 269–274.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, E. W., & Abramson, L. Y. (1983). Cognitive patterns and major depressive disorder: A longitudinal study in a hospital setting.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Marks, T., deMayo, R., & Mayol, A. (1985). Self-schemas and risks for depression: A prospective study.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59 1147–1159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heise, D. R. (1965). Semantic differential profiles for 1,000 most frequent English words.Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 79 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & King, G. (1981). Accessibility of social constructs: Consequences of individual and contextual variability. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.),Personality, cognition, and social interaction (pp. 69–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognitions in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95 52–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., & Reed, M. R. (1986). Information encoding and retrieval processes in depression: Findings, issues, and future directions. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.),Information processing approaches to clinical psychology (pp. 131–150). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. J., Russell, W. A., & Suci, G. J. (1958). An atlas of semantic profiles for 360 words.American Journal of Psychology, 71 688–699.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., MacDonald, M. R., & Derry, P. A. (1983). Parameters of a depressive self-schema. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.),Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 22). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., Olinger, L. J., & MacDonald, M. R. (1988). Processing personal and social information: The role of vulnerability and episodic schemata in depression. In. L. B. Alloy (Ed.),Cognitive processes in depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J. L., Larson, D. W., & Franklin, J. (1981). Depression-related cognitions: Antecedent or consequence?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 213–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1986). Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95 131–138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, J. D., Kessler, R. C., & Landis, K. R. (1992). Speed of recovery from major depressive episodes in a community sample of married men and women.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101 277–286.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, J., & Persons, J. B. (1988). Dysfunctional attitudes are mood state dependent.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97 251–264.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, J., Persons, J. B., & Byers, C. N. (1990). Endorsement of dysfunctional beliefs depends on current mood state.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97 251–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, J. (1980).Adjectives rated as self-descriptive by depressed and manic psychiatric patients. Unpublished manuscript, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, S. L., & Sarason, I. G. (1981). Emotion, personality, and selective attention.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 945–960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Persons, J. B., & Rao, P. A. (1985). Longitudinal study of cognitions, life events, and depression in psychiatric inpatients.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94 51–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J. E., & Monroe, S. M. (1992). Vulnerable self-esteem and depressive symptoms: Prospective findings comparing three alternative conceptualizations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 804–812.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1990). Are people changed by the experience of having an episode of depression: A further test of the scar hypothesis.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99 264–271.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sacco, W. P., & Beck, A. T. (1985). Cognitive therapy of depression. In E. E. Beckham and W. R. Leber (Eds.),Handbook of depression: Treatment, assessment, and research (pp. 3–38). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression.Psychological Bulletin, 103 147–162.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, B. F., Vallis, T. M., & McCabe, S. B. (1985). The assessment of the severity and symptom patterns in depression. In E. E. Beckham and W. R. Leber (Eds.),Handbook of depression: Treatment, assessment, and research (pp. 372–407). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, J. S., Silverman, J. A., & Eardley, D. A. (1984). Do maladaptive attitudes cause depression?Archives of General Psychiatry, 41 28–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielman, L. A., & Bargh, J. A. (1990). Does the depressive self-schema really exits? In C. D. McCann & N. S. Endler (Eds.),Depression: New directions in theory, research, and practice (pp. 111–126). Toronto: Wall & Emerson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, R., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1978). Research diagnostic criteria: Rationale and reliability.Archives of General Psychiatry, 35 733–782.

    Google Scholar 

  • Straube, E. R., & Germer, C. K. (1979). Dichotic shadowing and selective attention to word meaning in schizophrenia.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88 346–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression.Cognition and Emotion, 2 247–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97 346–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978).Development and validation of the dysfunctional attitude scale: A preliminary investigation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The research reported in this paper was facilitated by a Senior Research Fellowship from the Ontario Mental Health Foundation to Ian H. Gotlib. The authors express their appreciation to Dr. Valerie Whiffen and Cheryl McCabe for serving as second raters in the reliability aspects of this study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McCabe, S.B., Gotlib, I.H. Attentional processing in clinically depressed subjects: A longitudinal investigation. Cogn Ther Res 17, 359–377 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01177660

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01177660

Key words

Navigation