Skip to main content
Log in

Goal specification alters perceived pain intensity and tolerance latency

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The effects of instructing subjects to tolerate pain for a fixed amount of time (15 min) versus an unspecified amount of time (“last as long as you can”) was tested using an ischemic pain test. Subjects' latencies to terminate the painful stimulus and their perceived pain intensity ratings were recorded, both before and after cognitive strategy training. Strategy training helped subjects given fixed-time instructions decrease pain ratings and subjects given open-time instructions increase tolerance times. Nevertheless, subjects given specific time goals for tolerating the pain had lower absolute pain ratings and higher tolerance times than subjects given nonspecific goals, whether given strategy training or not. These results suggest that instructional set in and of itself can be therapeutic or deleterious in helping subjects cope with pain.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Avia, M. D., & Kanfer, F. H. (1980). Coping with aversive stimulation: The effects of training in a self-management context.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4 73–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45 1017–1028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., & Donnerstein, E. (1982). External validity is more than skin deep.American Psychologist, 37 245–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fordyce, W. E. (1976).Behavioral methods for chronic pain and illness. St. Louis: C. V. Mosby.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, G., & Rollman, G. B. (1983). The validity of experimental pain measures.Pain, 17 369–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanfer, F. H., & Goldfoot, D. A. (1966). Self-control and tolerance of noxious stimulation.Psychological Reports, 18 79–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanfer, F. H., & Seidner, M. L. (1973). Self-control: Factors enhancing tolerance of noxious stimulation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25 381–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karoly, P., & Jensen, M. P. (1987).Multimethod assessment of chronic pain. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1979). Nonspecific treatment factors in psychotherapy outcome research.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47 846–851.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1980).Research design in clinical psychology. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knuckle, C. (1949). Phasic pain induced by cold.Journal of Applied Psychology, 1 468–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S. F., Draugan, R. C., & Grau, J. W. (1982). Controllability, coping behavior, and stressinduced analgesia in the rat.Pain, 12 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melzack, R. (Ed.). (1983).Pain measurement and assessment. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postlethwaite, R., Grieve, N., Santacroce, T., Renfree, L., Wilson, G., & Peck, C. (1980). An analysis of pain produced by submaximum tourniquet test. In C. Peck & M. Wallace (Eds.),Problems in pain: Proceedings of the first Australia-New Zealand conference on pain (pp. 128–135). Sydney: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953).Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, G. M., Egbert, L. D., Markowitz, R. A., Mosteller, F., & Beecher, H. K. (1966). An experimental pain sensitive to morphine in man: The submaximal effort tourniquet technique.Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 154 324–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, G. M., Lowenstein, E., Hubbard, J. H., & Beecher, H. K. (1968). Experimental pain produced by the submaximal effort tourniquet technique: Further evidence of validity.Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 163 468–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spanos, N. P., Radtke-Bodorik, H. L., Ferguson, J. D., & Jones, D. (1979). The effects of hypnotic susceptibility, suggestions for analgesia, and the utilization of cognitive strategies on the reduction of pain.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88 282–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, R. J., & Heide, F. (1977). Analgesic characteristics of prepared childbirth techniques: Attention focusing and systematic relaxation.Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 21 429–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, M. K., Kanfer, F. H., & Higgins, J. M. (1984). Effects of goal specificity and time cues on pain tolerance.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8 415–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strayhorn, J. M. (1987). Control groups for psychological intervention outcome studies.American Journal of Psychiatry, 144 275–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. A., & Thorn, B. E. (1986). Can research methodology affect treatment outcome? A comparison of two cold pressor test paradigms.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10 539–546.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This study was the basis for an undergraduate honors thesis by the second author, Department of Psychology, Ohio State University.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Thorn, B.E., Williams, G.A. Goal specification alters perceived pain intensity and tolerance latency. Cogn Ther Res 13, 171–183 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173271

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173271

Key words

Navigation