Skip to main content
Log in

Nonverbal expectancy violations and conversational involvement

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A central feature of nonverbal expectancy violations theory is that unexpected behaviors trigger a cognitive-affective appraisal of such behavior, leading to a valencing of the behavior(s) as positive or negative. It has been proposed that communicator reward mediates the interpretation and evaluation of such violations but may be more important when the violative act is ambiguous in meaning than when it is not. Unclear is whether nonverbal behavioral composites introduce greater or less equivocality of interpretation. Two experiments employing multi-cue conversational involvement violations addressed this issue. In the first, dyads (N=51) engaged in a 10-minute baseline interview, after which participants rated each other on several measures of reward valence. One randomly selected member then served as a confederate interviewee during a second interview and either significantly increased or decreased involvement. In the second experiment, reward was manipulated as physical attractiveness, status, and task expertise. Dyads (N = 60) engaged in prolonged problem-solving discussions during which the confederates either committed an involvement violation or not. In both experiments, the involvement changes were sufficiently unexpected, arousing, and distracting to qualify as violations of expectations. Analysis of message interpretations indicated that (1) relative to normal involvement levels, increased nonverbal involvement was interpreted as most immediate/affectionate, receptive, similar, dominant, and composed, and decreased involvement as least so, and (2) reward mediated only the interpretation of formality. High involvement violations in turn produced greater attraction, credibility, and persuasiveness than low involvement violations for high as well as low-reward communicators, as predicted.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy in interpersonal communication. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.),Multichannel integration of nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976).Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Backman, C. W., & Secord, P. F. (1959). The effect of perceived liking on interpersonal attraction.Human Relations, 12 379–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. H. (1969).Interpersonal attraction, Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandt, D. R. (1979). On linking social performance with social competence: Some relations between communicative style and attributions of interpersonal attractiveness and effectiveness.Human Communication Research, 5 223–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model of personal space violations: Explication and an initial test.Human Communication Research, 4, 129–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1983). Nonverbal violation of expectations. In J. M. Wiemann & R. P. Harrison (Eds.),Nonverbal interaction (pp. 77–111). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1985).Expectancies, rewards, violations and outcomes: Applications to the instructional setting. Paper presented to the International Communication Association convention, Honolulu, HI.

  • Burgoon, J. K. & Aho, L. (1982). Three field experiments on the effects of violations of conversational distance.Communication Monographs, 49, 71–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., Hale, J. L. & deTurck, M. A. (1984). Relational messages associated with nonverbal behaviors.Human Communication Research, 10, 351–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. & Hale, J. L. (1984). The fundamental topoi of relational communication.Communication Monographs, 51, 193–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Coker, D. A., & Coker, R. A. (1986). Communicative effects of gaze behavior: A test of two contrasting explanations.Human Communication Research, 12, 495–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. & Hale, J. L. (1987). Validation and measurement of the fundamental themes of relational communication.Communication Monographs, 54, 19–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal expectancy violations: Model elaboration and application to immediacy behaviors.Communication Monographs, 55, 58–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. & Jones, S. B. (1976). Toward a theory of personal space expectations and their violations.Human Communication Research, 2 131–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Kelley, D. L., Newton, D. A., & Keeley-Dyreson, M. P. (1989). The nature of arousal and nonverbal indices. Manuscript submitted for publication.Human Communication Research, 1b, 217–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Manusev, V., Minee, P. & Hale, J. L. (1985). Effects of eye gaze on hiring, credibility attraction and relational message interpretation.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 133–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Olney, C. A. (1989).Effects of gender on nonverbal expectancies and relational message interpretations of nonverbal behavior. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Burgoon, J. K., Olney, C. A., & Coker, R. A. (1987). The effects of communicator characteristics on patterns of reciprocity and compensation.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 146–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Stacks, D. W., & Burch, S. A. (1982). The role of interpersonal rewards and violations of distancing expectations in achieving influence in small groups.Communication, Journal of the Communication Association of the Pacific, 11, 114–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Stacks, D. W., & Woodall, W. G. (1979). A communicative model of violations of distancing expectations.Western Journal of Speech Communication, 43 153–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1983). Conversational involvement: Approaching and avoiding others. In J. M. Wiemann & R. P. Harrison (Eds.),Nonverbal interaction (pp. 113–148). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N., & Green, J. O. (1982). A discrepancy-arousal explanation of mutual influence in expressive behavior in adult-adult and infant-adult interaction.Communication Monographs, 49, 89–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbone, T. (1975). Stylistic variables as related to source credibility.Speech Monographs, 42, 99–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cegala, D. J., Savage, G. T., Brunner, C. C., & Conrad, A. B. (1982). An elaboration of the meaning of interaction involvement: Toward the development of a theoretical concept.Communication Monographs, 49, 229–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cegala, D. J., Wall, V. D., & Rippey, G. (1987). An investigation of interaction involvement and the dimensions of SYMLOG: Perceived communication behaviors of persons in task-oriented groups.Central States Speech Journal, 38, 81–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coker, D. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). The nature of conversational involvement and nonverbal encoding patterns.Human Communication Research, 13, 463–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coutts, L. M., Schneider, F. W., & Montgomery, S. (1980). An investigation of the arousal model of interpersonal intimacy.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 545–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, H. S., DiMatteo, M. R., & Tatanta, A. (1980). A study of the relationship between individual differences in nonverbal expressiveness and factors of personality and social interaction.Journal of Research in Personality, 14, 351–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galle, A., Spratt, G., Chapman, A. J., & Smallbone, A. (1975). EEG correlates of eye contact and interpersonal distance.Biological Psychology, 3, 237–245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCroskey, J. C., Jensen, T., & Valencia, C. (1973, May).Measurement of the credibility of peers and spouses. Papers presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association, Montreal.

  • McCroskey, J. C., & McCain, T. A. (1974). The measurement of interpersonal attraction.Speech Monographs, 41, 261–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, M. L., & Cody, M. J. (1982). Awkward silences: Behavioral antecedents and consequences of the conversational lapse.Human Communication Research, 8, 299–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1983).Nonverbal behavior: A functional perspective. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. J., & Knowles, E. S. (1979). Affective and cognitive mediators of reactions to spatial invasions.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 437–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzberg, B. H. & Cupach, W. R. (1984).Interpersonal communication competence. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stacks, D. W., & Burgoon, J. K. (1981). The role of nonverbal behaviors as distractors in resistance to persuasion in interpersonal contexts.Central States Speech Journal, 32, 61–73.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

where Joe Walther and Jim Baesler are doctoral students. An earlier version of this paper was presented to the annual meeting of the International Communication Association, New Orleans, May 1988.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burgoon, J.K., Newton, D.A., Walther, J.B. et al. Nonverbal expectancy violations and conversational involvement. J Nonverbal Behav 13, 97–119 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00990793

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00990793

Keywords

Navigation