Skip to main content
Log in

The role of lexical stress in word recognition and pronunciation

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

In the present paper several studies are reviewed that are relevant to lexical-stress assignment. The questions asked concern the form of representation of stress and its role in the perception and identification of words in auditory and visual word recognition. The role of stress is also considered in the computation of phonology, as are the linguistic and orthographic constraints that influence its assignment during reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. (1981). What good is orthographic redundancy? In H. Singer & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.),Perception of print (pp. 192–221). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D. A. (1984). Speech production and comprehension: One lexicon or two? In W. Prinz & A. Sanders (Eds.),Cognition and motor processes (pp. 209–228). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, P. T., & Smith, R. G. (1976). A psycholinguistic model of English assignment rules.Language and Speech, 19, 9–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1985). The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: Lexical access and/or production?Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 89–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. W., & Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Lexical access and the spelling-to-sound regularity effect.Memory & Cognition, 8, 424–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., & Johnston, J. (1989). Reading and the mental lexicon. On the uptake of visual information. In W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.),Lexical representation and process (pp. 291–316). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, M., & Byng, S. (1986). Prosodic constraints in lexical access in reading.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 369–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, M., & Byng, S. (1989). Re-stressing prosody: A reply to Cutler, Howard and Patterson.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 85–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombo, L. (1988). Influence of stress assignment in reading Italian words.Contributi dei Dipartimenti e degli Istituti Italiani di Psicologia, 1, 2, 69–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.),Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Besner, D., Jonasson, J. T., & Davelaar, E. (1979). Phonological encoding in the lexical decision task.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 489–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connine, C. M., Clifton, C. J., & Cutler, A. (1987). Effects of lexical stress on phonetic categorization.Phonetica, 44, 133–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1980). Errors of stress and intonation. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.),Errors in. Linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand (pp. 167–180). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1986). Forbear is a homophone: Lexical prosody does not constrain lexical access.Language & Speech, 29, 201–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1989). Auditory lexical access: Where do we start? In W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.),Lexical representation and process (pp. 342–356). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Clifton, C. E. (1984). The use of prosodic information in word recognition. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.),Attention and performance (Vol. X, pp. 183–196). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1989). Misplaced stress on prosody: A reply to Black and Byng.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 67–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Isard, S. (1980). The production of prosody. In B. Butterworth (Ed.),Language production. Speech and talk (Vol. 1, pp. 229–269). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Norris, D. (1989). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 113–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production.Psychological Review, 93, 283–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Even, L. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1981). The use of abstract graphemic information in lexical access.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33 A, 325–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organisation and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F., & Gee, J. P. (1987). Prosodic structure and spoken word recognition. In U. H. Frauenfelder & L. Komisarjevski Tyler (Eds.),Spoken word recognition (pp. 135–155). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. (1985). Issues in the modelling of pronunciation assembly in normal reading. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.),Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 259–308). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Priming effects with phonemically similar words: The encoding hypothesis reconsidered.Memory & Cognition, 8, 115–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., & Evett, L. J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dualroute theory of reading.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 689–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, J., & Bishop, D. (1987). Anatomical differences between nose, palm and foot, or, the body in question: Further dissection of the processes of sub-lexical spelling-sound translation. In M. Coltheart (Ed.),Attention & Performance (Vol. XII, pp. 419–469). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, J., & Marcel, A. J. (1981). One process, not two, in reading aloud: Lexical analogies do the work of non lexical rules.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33 A, 397–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, M. H., & Bock, J. K. (1988). Stress in time.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 389–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudanna, A., Burani, C., Cermele, A., & Parisi, D. (1989). Effetti sillabici e accentuali nella lettura di parole nuove.Giornale italiano di psicologia XVI, 119–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, M., & Prince, A. S. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm.Linguistic Inguiry, 8, 249–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, G., Feldman, L. B., Turvey, M. T., Carello, C., & Katz, L. (1989). Alphabet priming in Bi-alphabetical word perception.Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 237–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phonemic similarity effects and prelexical phonology.Memory & Cognition, 18, 128–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCann, R. S., Besner, D., & Davelaar, E. (1988). Word recognition and identification. Do word-frequency effects reflect lexical access?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 693–706.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (1987). On the relation between lexical input and output pathways for speech. In A. Allport, D. MacKay, W. Prinz, & E. Scheerer (Eds.),Language perception and production: Relationships between listening, speaking, reading and writing (pp. 273–312). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S., Doyle, M. C., & Haggard, P. N. (1989). Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: Where are they?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 43–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, G. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition.Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., & Patterson, K. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation or an attempt at a new interpretation. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.),Deep dyslexia (pp. 91–118). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkin, A. J. (1982). Phonological recoding in lexical decision: Effects of spelling-to-sound regularity depend on how regularity is defined.Memory & Cognition, 10, 43–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., & Coltheart, V. (1987). Phonological processes in reading. A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.),Attention & Performance (Vol. XII, pp. 421–447). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., & Morton, J. (1985). From orthography to phonology: An attempt at an old interpretation. In K. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.),Surface dyslexia: Neuropsyehological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 335–359). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S. S., & Rubenstein, M. A. (1971). Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 645–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1985a). The time-course of phonological code activation in two writing systems.Cognition, 19, 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1985b). The time course of information activation and utilization in visual word recognition. In D. Besner, T. G. Waller, & G. E. MacKinnon (Eds.),Reading research (Vol. X, pp. 199–252). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1987). Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy? In M. Coltheart (Ed.),Attention & Performance (Vol. XII, pp. 245–263). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed developmental model of word recognition.Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Leiman, J. M., & Bienkowski, M. (1982). Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing.Cognitive Psychology, 14, 489–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition?Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 383–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, E. O. (1984).Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1983). Sublexical units and suprasegmental structure in speech production planning. In P. F. MacNeilage (Ed.),The production of speech (pp. 109–136). New York, Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, H. G., Hornak, R., & Sanders, E. (1978). The effects of graphemic, phonetic, and semantic relationships on access to lexical structure.Memory & Cognition, 6, 115–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D. A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (RE)consideration of context effects.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645–659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound and reading.Memory & Cognition, 15, 181–198.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Colombo, L. The role of lexical stress in word recognition and pronunciation. Psychol. Res 53, 71–79 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00867334

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00867334

Keywords

Navigation