Skip to main content
Log in

Adjustment and readjustment of the relative timing of a motor pattern

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

The hypothesis of a generalized motor program with relative timing as an invariant characteristic is contrasted with a “continuity hypothesis”. According to this hypothesis, temporal characteristics are determined by the same parameters of a generalized motor program as spatial characteristics are; different temporal patterns are assumed to be associated with different costs. In two experiments uniarticular arm movements with different relative timing were studied. Three results were obtained that support the continuity hypothesis. (1) Reproductions exhibited asymmetric biases, that is, reproductions of one target pattern were biased toward the other, but not vice versa. (2) Readjustment to a new target pattern that had a correlation of almost zero with the practice pattern was not immediate, as found by Heuer and Schmidt (1988), who used more similar patterns. This suggests that readjustment of a temporal pattern depends on the similarity between the old and the new patterns. (3) Readjustment to another target pattern that had been practiced before was immediate for similar patterns, but not immediate for dissimilar patterns. The effects of the similarity of different temporal patterns on readjustment are not consistent with the view that each temporal pattern is governed by its own generalized motor program.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armstrong, T. R. (1970). Training for the production of memorized movement patterns. Technical Report No. 26, Human Performance Center, University of Michigan.

  • Bizzi, E., & Polit, A. (1979). Characteristics of the motor programs underlying visually evoked movements. In R. E. Talbott & D. R. Humphrey (Eds.), Posture and movement. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, M. C., & Shapiro, D. C. (1984). Control of sequential movements: Evidence for generalized motor programs. Journal of Neurophysiology, 52, 787–796.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craske, B., & Craske, J. D. (1985). Muscular after-contraction reveals oscillator mechanisms in the human motor apparatus. Human Movement Science, 4, 249–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craske, B., & Craske, J. D. (1986). Oscillator mechanisms in the human motor system: Investigating their properties using the after-contraction effect. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 117–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruse, H. (1986). Constraints for joint angle control of the human arm. Biological Cybernetics, 54, 125–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruse, H., & Brüwer, M. (1987). The human arm as a redundant manipulator: The control of path and joint angles. Biological Cybernetics, 57, 137–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derwort, A. (1938). Untersuchungen über den Zeitverlauf figurierter Bewegungen beim Menschen. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 240, 661–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fel'dman, A. G. (1966). Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. — II. Controllable parameters of the muscles. Biophysics, 11, 565–578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fel'dman, A. G. (1974). Change in the length of the muscle as a consequence of a shift in equilibrium in the muscle-load system. Biophysics, 19, 544–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. R. (1982). Evidence against a central control model of typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 753–810.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. R. (1987). Timing of skilled motor performance: Tests of the proportional duration model. Psychological Review, 94, 255–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, H. (1979). Über Bewegungsprogramme bei willkürlichen Bewegungen. Berichte aus dem Fachbereich Psychologie der Philipps-Universität Marburg/Lahn Nr. 76.

  • Heuer, H. (1984). On re-scaleability of force and time in aiming movements. Psychological Research, 46, 73–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, H. (1985). Some points of contact between models of central capacity and factor analytic models. Acta Psychologica, 60, 135–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, H. (in press). Testing the invariance of relative timing. A comment on Gentner (1987). Psychological Review.

  • Heuer, H., & Schmidt, R. A. (1988). Transfer of learning among motor patterns with different relative timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 241–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holst, E. von (1937). Vom Wesen der Ordnung im Zentralnerven-system. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 625–631, 641–647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, I. (1951). Über Aufbau und Wandlungen der Wahrnehmungswelt, insbesondere über “bedingte Empfindungen”. Sitzungsberichte der österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften, 227. Band, 1. Abhandlung. Wien: Rohrer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley, D. J., & Zelaznik, H. N. (1984). The acquisition of time properties associated with a sequential motor skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 275–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. The Hixon Symposium. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marteniuk, R. G., MacKenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S., & Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 365–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness. The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1985). Motor programming: A review and scheduling theory. In H. Heuer, U. Kleinbeck, & K. H. Schmidt (Eds.), Motor behavior. Programming, control, and acquisition. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1980). On the theoretical status of time in motor program representations. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 145–166). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1985). The search for invariance in skilled movement behavior. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 52, 188–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, D. C. (1977). A preliminary attempt to determine the duration of a motor program. In D. M. Landers & R. W. Christina (Eds.), Psychology of motor behavior and sport I. Champaign, Ill.: Human Kinetics Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, D. C., Zernicke, R. F., Gregor, R. J., & Diestel, J. D. (1981). Evidence for generalized motor programs using gait pattern analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 13, 33–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sturgis, S. P. (1983). A spectral analysis tutorial with examples in FORTRAN. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 15, 377–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Summers, J. J. (1975). The role of timing in motor program representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 229–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Summers, J. J., Sargent, G. I., & Hawkins, S. R. (1984). Rhythm and the timing of movement sequences. Psychological Research, 46, 107–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terzuolo, C. A., & Viviani, P. (1979). The central representation of learned motor patterns. In R. E. Talbott & D. R. Humphrey (Eds.), Posture and movement (pp. 113–121). New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terzuolo, C. A., & Viviani, P. (1980). Determinants and characteristics of patterns used for typing. Neuroscience, 5, 1085–1103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viviani, P. (1986). Do units of motor action really exist? In H. Heuer & C. Fromm (Eds.), Generation and modulation of action patterns (pp. 201–216). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viviani, P., Campadelli, P., & Mounoud, P. (1987). Visuo-manual pursuit tracking of human two-dimensional movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 62–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. A. (1980). Space-time invariance in learned motor skills. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 525–533). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch, R. B. (1978). Perceptual modification. Adapting to altered sensory environments. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G., & Schmidt, R. A. (in press). Variability in practice: Facilitation in retention and transfer through schema formation or context effects? Journal of Motor Behavior.

  • Zelaznik, H. N., Schmidt, R. A., & Gielen, S. C. A. M. (1986). Kinematic properties of aimed hand movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 353–372.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Heuer, H. Adjustment and readjustment of the relative timing of a motor pattern. Psychol. Res 50, 83–93 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309207

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309207

Keywords

Navigation