Skip to main content
Log in

Different trends in perceptual pattern microgenesis as a function of the spatial range of local brightness averaging

Towards an empirical method for the differentiation between global and local levels of form as related to processing in real time

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

Stimulus patterns, quantized into small squares with the brightness level averaged within each square, were presented for recognition with various exposure durations. Qualitatively different recognition functions were obtained with varying spatial levels of quantization. The method used, if further elaborated, seems appropriate for the empirical differentiation between global and local levels of form and between integration and differentiation stages of a perceptual/attentional process.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bachmann, T. (1984). The process of perceptual retouch: Nonspecific afferent activation dynamics in explaining visual masking. Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 69–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breitmeyer, B. G. (1984). Visual masking: An integrative approach. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1977). The hidden preattentive processes. American Psychologist, 32, 109–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caelli, T., & Yuzyk, J. (1985). What is perceived when two images are combined? Perception, 14, 41–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calis, G., Sterenborg, J., & Maarse, F. (1984). Initial microgenetic steps in single-glance face recognition. Acta Psychologica, 55, 215–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falzett, M., & Lappin, J. S. (1983). Detection of visual forms in space and time. Vision Research, 23, 181–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Froehlich, W. D., Smith, G., Draguns, J. G., & Hentschel, U. (Eds.) (1984). Psychological processes in cognition and personality. Washington: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gubler, E. V. (1978). Computational methods of analysis and recognition of pathological processes (in Russian). Leningrad: Meditsina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon, L. D., & Julesz, B. (1973). Masking in visual recognition: Effects of two-dimensional filtered noise. Science, 180, 1194–1197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. S. (1980). Visual coding and adaptability. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, G. (1976). Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception. Psychological Research, 38, 379–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. (1980). Spatial-frequency channels in one-, two-, and three-dimensional vision: Variation on an auditory theme by Bekesy. In C. S. Harris (Ed.), Visual Coding and Adaptability (pp. 263–316). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. (1981). Figure and ground perception in briefly presented isodipole textures. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual Organization (pp. 27–54). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. (1979). The order of visual processing: “Top-down”, “bottom-up”, or “middle-out”? Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 225–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeuwenberg, E., Mens, L., & Calis, G. (1985). Knowledge within perception: Masking caused by incompatible interpretation. Acta Psychologica, 59, 91–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lomov, B. F. (1966). Man and machine (in Russian). Moscow: Sovetskoye Radio.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nikitin, M. P. (1905). On the problem of formation of visual perceptions. An experimental investigation (in Russian). Vestnik psikhologii, kriminal'noy antropologii i gipnotizma, 2, 112–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersik, J. T. (1978). Possible role of transient and sustained visual mechanisms in the determination of similarity judgments. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 683–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, W. R. (1975). An autocorrelation theory of form detection. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, L. M. (1982). Determinants of attention to local and global features of visual form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 562–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1935). Studies on contour: I. Qualitative analyses. American Journal of Psychology, 47, 40–64.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bachmann, T. Different trends in perceptual pattern microgenesis as a function of the spatial range of local brightness averaging. Psychol. Res 49, 107–111 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308675

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308675

Keywords

Navigation