Skip to main content
Log in

On the computational architecture of the neocortex

I. The role of the thalamo-cortical loop

  • Published:
Biological Cybernetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper proposes that each area of the cortex carries on its calculations with the active participation of a nucleus in the thalamus with which it is reciprocally and topographically connected. Each cortical area is responsible for maintaining and updating the organism's knowledge of a specific aspect of the world, ranging from low level raw data to high level abstract representations, and involving interpreting stimuli and generating actions. In doing this, it will draw on multiple sources of expertise, learned from experience, creating multiple, often conflicting, hypotheses which are integrated by the action of the thalamic neurons and then sent back to the standard input layer of the cortex. Thus this nucleus plays the role of an ‘active blackboard’ on which the current best reconstruction of some aspect of the world is always displayed. Evidence for this theory is reviewed and experimental tests are proposed. A sequel to this paper will discuss the cortico-cortical loops and propose quite different computational roles for them.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bender DB (1981) Retinotopic organization of the macaque pulvinar. J Neurophys 46:672–693

    Google Scholar 

  • Blinkov SM, Glazer II (1968) The human brain in figures and tables. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford J, Barclay JB, Franks J (1972) Sentence memory: a constructure vs. interpretive approach. Cogn Psychol 3:193–209

    Google Scholar 

  • Brodal A (1981) Neurological anatomy. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherniak C (1990) The bounded brain. J Cogn Neurosci 2:58–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark J, Yuille A (1990) Data fusion for sensory information processing systems. Kluwer Academic Press, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick F (1984) Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 81:4586–4590

    Google Scholar 

  • Erman LD, Hayes-Roth F, Lesser VR, Reddy R (1980) The HEARSAY-II speech understanding system. Comput Surv 12:213–253

    Google Scholar 

  • Evarts EV (1973) Motor cortex reflexes associated with learned movement. Science 179:501–503

    Google Scholar 

  • Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex: (to be published)

  • Gemba H, Hashimoto S, Sasaki K (1981) Cortical field potentials preceding visually initiated hand movements in monkeys. Exp Brain Res 42:435–441

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos AP, Lurito JT, Petrides M, Schwartz AB, Massey JT (1989) Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science 243:234–236

    Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel AM, Berson DM (1981) Families of related cortical areas in the extrastriate visual system. In: Cortical sensory organization, vol 2. Humana Press, Clifton, NJ, pp 103–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Harth E (1983) Windows on the mind. Quill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Harth E, Unnikrishnan KP, Pandya AS (1987) The inversion of sensory processing by feedback pathways: a model of visual cognitive functions. Science 1987:184–187

    Google Scholar 

  • Jahnsen H, Llinas R (1984) Electrophysiological properties of guineapig thalamic neurons: an in vitro study. J Physiol London 349:205–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones EB (1985) The thalamus. Plenum Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsey P, Norman D (1977) Human information processing. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria AR (1969) Higher cognitive functions in man, 2nd edn. Moscow University Press, Moscow (English edn 1980, Basic Books, New York)

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr D (1982) Vision. Freeman, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky M (1975) A framework for representing knowledge. In: Winston P (ed) The psychology of computer vision. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mjolness E, Gindi G, Anandan P (1988) Optimization in model matching and perceptual organization. Research report YaleU/ DCS/RR-634

  • Ojemann G (1983) Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping. Behav Brain Sci 2:189–206

    Google Scholar 

  • Rockel AJ, Hiorns RW, Powell TPS (1980) The basic uniformity in structure of the neocortex. Brain 103:221–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Selfridge O (1959) Pandemonium: a paradigm for learning. In: Symposium on the Mechanization of Thought Processes. HM Stationary Office, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd G (1990) The synaptic organization of the brain, 3rd edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman SM, Koch C (1986) The control of retinogeniculate transmission in the mammalian lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp Brain Res 63:1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade M, Llinas RR (1988) The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. Physiol Rev 68:649–742

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade M, Domich L, Oakson G (1986) Reticularis thalami neurons revisited: activity changes during shifts in states of vigilance. J Neurosci 6:68–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman A (1988) Features and objects. Q J Exp Psychol 40A:1988

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman S (1984) Visual routines. Cognition 18:97–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heydt R, Peterhans E (1989) Cortical contour mechanisms and geometrical illusions. In: Lam DM, Gilbert CD (eds) Neural mechanisms of visual perception. Gulf, Houston, Texas

    Google Scholar 

  • Weller RE, Kaas JH (1981) Cortical and subcortical connections of visual cortex in primates. In: Cortical sensory organization, vol 2. Humana Press, Clifton, NJ, pp 121–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Winston P (1975) Learning structural descriptions from examples. In: Winston (ed) The psychology of computer vision. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mumford, D. On the computational architecture of the neocortex. Biol. Cybern. 65, 135–145 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00202389

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00202389

Keywords

Navigation