Skip to main content

Transitions to Intentional and Symbolic Communication in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Prelinguistic and Minimally Verbal Communicators on the Autism Spectrum

Abstract

We begin by reviewing research focused on the way in which the emergence of new forms of intentional and symbolic communication alters the typically developing child’s communicative environment. Our central thesis is that these alterations not only change the nature of the input that the child receives but also influence the availability of opportunities for learning that support future development. We then review what is known about delays and atypicalities in the development of intentional and symbolic communication in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Based on these data, we suggest that these communicative delays and atypicalities have far-reaching, cascading effects that extend beyond the individuals themselves to impact the behavior of social partners, the communicative environment more broadly, and the course of subsequent development. We then present a conceptual framework that identifies ways in which delays in the emergence of basic, early emerging communicative behaviors – eye contact, gesture, and vocalization – may lead to delays in the emergence of the individual’s ability to initiate instances of joint attention and impact the caregiver’s sense of the child’s developmental level. These changes in turn may lead to a reduction in shared topics for communication and, therefore, to a reduction in instances in which linguistic input adapted to moments of shared attention is most effective in facilitating the early development of language. Finally, we conclude with some recommendations for research and clinical practice suggested by this framework.

Author Note

Preparation of this chapter was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (R01 HD073255) to JMI. We are grateful to Nina Leezenbaum, Jessie Northrup, and Kelsey West for discussion of the conceptual framework presented here. We would also like to dedicate this chapter to Virginia Volterra and to the memory of Elizabeth Bate s. Their groundbreaking research and the mentorship that they provided the first author laid the foundation for this work.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Although there is some work on symbolic and representational gestures and their development between 9 and 12 months of age (e.g., Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; see Capone & McGregor, 2004, for a review), and children exposed to a sign language from early in life readily acquire language in the manual modality (e.g., Meier & Newport, 1990), we focus our discussion of the transition to symbolic communication on the emergence of words.

References

  • Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L. B., & Frick, J. E. (2003). The still face: A history of a shared experimental paradigm. Infancy, 4, 451–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 59–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., Rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Attwood, A., Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The understanding and use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and Down’s Syndrome Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 241–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, A. E. (2003). The development of joint attention in blind infants. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 259–275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. (1993). Language development from two to three. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, N. C., Fleming, K., Thiemann-Bourque, K., Olswang, L., Dowden, P., Saunders, M. D., & Marquis, J. (2012). Development of the communication complexity scale. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 21, 16–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in infant-mother interaction (pp. 271–289). New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., Parrini, B., & Cesari, A. (2003). The communicative use of pointing in autism: Developmental profile and factors related to change. European Psychiatry, 18, 6–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996). Communicative gestures during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 645–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capone, N., & McGregor, K. K. (2004). Gesture development: A review for clinical and research practices. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 47, 173–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delays. Autism, 2, 325–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 91–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colgan, S. E., Lanter, E., McComish, C., Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., & Baranek, G. T. (2006). Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 12, 307–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 281–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B. L., & MacNeilage, P. J. (1995). The articulatory basis of babbling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1199–1211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. (2000). Case study of the development of an infant with autism from birth to two years of age. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 299–313.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., & Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of nine to fifteen week-old human infants during face-to-face interactions with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–1279.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Stevenson, J. L., Khandakar, S., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2008). Why does joint attention look atypical in autism? Child Development Perspectives, 2, 38–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. M. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10, 778–785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2010). From birds to words: Perception of structure in social interactions guides vocal development and language learning. In M. S. Blumberg, J. H. Freeman, & S. R. Robinson (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of developmental and comparative neuroscience (pp. 708–729). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child Development, 80, 636–644.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J., Briesch, J., & Syal, S. (2010). Learning while babbling: Prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness to learn. Infancy, 15, 362–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). ‘I beg your pardon?’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language, 13, 455–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., Goldstein, M. H., & King, A. P. (2006). Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelinguistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 509–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gros‐Louis, J., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2014). Maternal responsiveness and the development of directed vocalizing in social interactions. Infancy, 19, 385–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., García-Pérez, R. M., & Lee, A. (2010). Person-centered (deictic) expressions and autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 403–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1987). Topic relations in mother-child conversation. First Language, 7, 145–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 229–261.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Tencer, H. L., Lany, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). The relation between gesture and speech in congenitally blind and sighted language-learners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 105–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Thal, D. J. (1998). Communicative transition: There’s more to the hand than meets the eye. In A. M. Wetherby, S. F. Warren, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic communication (pp. 59–86). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 19–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. J., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., & Loveland, K. A. (1989). The effect of social context on the functional communication skills of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 283–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leezenbaum, N. B., Campbell, S. B., Butler, D., & Iverson, J. M. (2014). Maternal verbal responses to communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism. Autism, 18, 694–703.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of an interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., … Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule – Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 9, 23–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masur, E. F. (1990). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. In V. Volterra & C. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 18–30). Berlin, Germany/New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, R. P., & Newport, E. L. (1990). Out of the hands of babes: On a possible sign advantage in language acquisition. Language, 66, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. A., Cohn, J. F., & Campbell, S. B. (2001). Infant affective responses to mother’s still face at 6 months differentially predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors at 18 months. Developmental Psychology, 37, 706–714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Miami, FL: University of Miami.

    Google Scholar 

  • Northrup, J. B., & Iverson, J. M. (2015). Vocal coordination during early parent-infant interactions predicts language outcome in infant siblings of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Infancy, 20, 523–547.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96, B101–B113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. -M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Moore Hill, M., Hutman, T., … Young, G. S. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 256–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parladé, M. V., & Iverson, J. M. (2011). The interplay between language, gesture, and affect during communicative transition: A dynamic systems approach. Developmental Psychology, 47, 820–833.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Parladé, M. V., & Iverson, J. M. (2015). The development of coordinated communication in infants at heightened risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 2218–2234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patten, E., Belardi, K., Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Labban, J. D., & Oller, D. K. (2014). Vocal patterns in infants with autism spectrum disorder: Canonical babbling status and vocalization frequency. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2413–2428.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, J., & Schelde, T. (1997). Behavioral aspects of infantile autism: An ethological description. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, 96–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L., Bascome, A., Lampard, J., & Feeny, N. (2001). Conversational patterns in late talkers at age 3. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 235–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, D., Fein, D., Barton, M., & Green, J. (2001). The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT): An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 131–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheinkopf, S. J., Mundy, P., Oller, D. K., & Steffens, M. (2000). Vocal atypicalities of preverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 345–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1691–1704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T. (2004). Direction of regard and the still-face effect in the first year: Does intention matter? Child Development, 75, 468–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tarabulsy, G. M., Provost, M. A., Deslandes, J., St-Laurent, D., Moss, E., Lemelin, J., … Dassylva, J. (2003). Individual differences in infant still-face response at 6 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 421–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Töret, G., & Acarlar, F. (2011). Gestures in prelinguistic Turkish children with autism, Down syndrome, and typically developing children. Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice, 11, 1471–1478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Balkom, H., Verhoeven, L., & van Weerdenburg, M. (2010). Conversational behaviour of children with developmental language delay and their caretakers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, 295–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M. M., & McCune, L. (1994). When is a word a word? Journal of Child Language, 21, 517–542.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., Baranek, G. T., Dykstra, J. R., & Wilson, K. P. (2013). Communicative gesture use in infants with and without autism: A retrospective home video study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22, 25–39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364–377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., Cain, D., Yonclas, D., & Walker, V. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 240–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (2002). Communication and symbolic behavior scales: Developmental profile (1st normed ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winder, B. M., Wozniak, R. H., Parladé, M. V., & Iverson, J. M. (2013). Spontaneous initiation of communication in infants at low and heightened risk for autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 1931–1942.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Joint attention without gaze following: Human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination. PLOS ONE, e79659.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jana M. Iverson .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media Singapore

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Iverson, J.M., Wozniak, R.H. (2016). Transitions to Intentional and Symbolic Communication in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorder. In: Keen, D., Meadan, H., Brady, N., Halle, J. (eds) Prelinguistic and Minimally Verbal Communicators on the Autism Spectrum. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-0713-2_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics