Skip to main content

Validity and Reliability of the Experience-Sampling Method

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

To understand the dynamics of mental health, it is essential to develop measures for the frequency and the patterning of mental processes in every-day-life situations. The Experience-Sampling Method (ESM) is an attempt to provide a valid instrument to describe variations in self-reports of mental processes. It can be used to obtain empirical data on the following types of variables: (a) frequency and patterning of daily activity, social interaction, and changes in location; (b) frequency, intensity, and patterning of psychological states, i.e., emotional, cognitive, and conative dimensions of experience; (c) frequency and patterning of thoughts, including quality and intensity of thought disturbance. The article reviews practical and methodological issues of the ESM and presents evidence for its short-and long-term reliability when used as an instrument for assessing the variables outlined above.

This study was supported by the U.S. Public Health Service, the National Institute of Mental Health, and the Spencer Foundation. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 526–536. Copyright © 1987 Wolters Kluwer Health—Republished with permission.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Marta Wenger and colleagues of the Collaborative Study of Children with Special Needs at the Children’s Hospital Medical Center in Boston have used ESM with several samples of physically disabled children.

  2. 2.

    See footnote 1

  3. 3.

    Larson (1979) The significance of time alone in adolescents’ lives. Doctoral Dissertation, The University of Chicago.

  4. 4.

    Wells A (1985) Variations in self-esteem in the daily life of mothers, Doctoral Dissertion, The University of Chicago.

  5. 5.

    Hoover MD (1983) Individual differences in the relation of heart rate to self-reports. Doctoral Dissertation, The University of Chicago.

References

  • Altschuller, M. l. (1923). Byudzhet vremeni (Time Budgets). Perm: USSR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. (1955). Midwest and its children. New York: Row, Peterson, Evannton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G., Wright, H. F., Barker, L. S., & Schoggen, M. F. (1961). Specimen records of American and english children. Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, H. R., Killworth, P., Kronenfeld, D., & Sailer, L. (1984). On the validity of retrospective data: The problem of informant accuracy. Annual Review of Anthropology, 13, 495–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevans, G. E. (1913). How workingmen spend their spare time. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradburn, N. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandstatter, H. (1983). Emotional responses to other persona in everyday life situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 871–883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunawick, E. (Ed.). (1952). The conceptual framework of psychology. International encyclopedia of unified science (Vol. 1(10)). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buse, L., & Pawlik, K. (1984). Inter-Setting-Korrelationen und Setting-Personlichkelts-Weschelwirkungen: Ergebnisse einer Feldunter-suchung zur Konsistenz von Verhalten and Erleben. Zeitschrift fur Socialpyschologie, 15, 44–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • CsikuMntmihalyi, M., & Figurski, T. J. (1982). Self-awareness and aversive experience in everyday life. Journal of Personality, 50, 14–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1980). The experience of freedom in everyday life. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 401–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent: Conflict and growth in the teenage years. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 281–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • deVries, M. (1983). Temporal patterning of psychiatric symptoms. Vienna: World Psychiatric Association Congress.

    Google Scholar 

  • deVries, M. (1984). Temperament and infant mortality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1189–1194.

    Google Scholar 

  • deVries, M., Delespaul, P., Dijkman, C., & Theunissen, J. (1986). Temporal and situational aspects of severe mental disorders. Milan Angeli: L’Esperienza quotidiana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Larson, R. J., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). Person X situation interactions: Choices of situations and congruence response models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 592–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijkman, C. I. M., & deVries, M. W. (1987). The social ecology of anxiety: Theoretical and quantitative perspectives. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 550–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Divilbiss, J., & Self, P. (1978). Work analysis by random sampling. Bulletin of the Medical Library Association, 66, 19–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N. S., & Magnusson, D. (Eds.). (1976). Interactional psychology and personality. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K., & Simon, H. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 216–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filstead, W., Reich, W., Parrella, D., & Rossi, J. (1985). Using electronic pagers to monitor the process of recovery in alcoholics and drug abusers. Paper presented at the 34th international congress on alcohol, drug Abuse and Tobacco, Calgary, Alberta, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, D. (1971). Measuring the concept of personality. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franzoi, S. L., & Brewer, L. C. (1984) The experience of self-awareness and its relation to level of self-consciousness: An experiential sampling study. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 522–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, M., Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1980) Immediate experience and its recollection. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 32, 167–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giannino, S., Graef, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1979) Well-being and the perceived balance between opportunities and capabilities. Paper presented at the 87th convention of the American Psychiatric Association, New York, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graef, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Gianinno, S. (1983). Measuring intrinsic motivation in everyday life. Leisure Studies, 2, 155–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, A. (1985). Self-concept and life transitions in early adolescence. Paper presented at the biannual meeting of the society for research on child development, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurin, G., Veroff, J., & Field, S. (1960). Americans view their mental health. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J. A., Hater, R. J., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1984). Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 183–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harre, R., & Secord, P. F. (1972). The explanation of social behavior. Oxford: Blackwelt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hormuth, S. E. (1984). Transitions in commitments to roles and self-concept change; recreation as a paradigm. In V. L. Allen & E. van de Vliert (Eds.), Role transitions: Explorations and explanations. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmuth, S. E. (1985). Methaden fur psychologische Forschung im Pels (Diskussionspapier Nr 43). Heidelberg: Paychologischen Institut der Universitat Heidelberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hormuth, S. E. (1986). The sampling of experiences in situ. Journal of Personality, 54, 262–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T. (1979). Random sampling of cognitions and behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 103–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T. (1980). Validation and correlation of thought sampling with retrospective measures. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 235–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, A. (1973). Time allocation in a machiquenga community. Ethology, 14, 301–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C., & Larson, R. (1982). Bulimia: An analysis of moods and behavior. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 341–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E., Barta, S., & Mexeimer, M. (1980). Motivational correlates of thought content frequency and commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1222–1237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. In H. Reis (Ed.), New directions for naturalistic methods in the behavioral sciences. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Freeman, M. (1984). Alcohol and marijuana use in adolescents’ daily lives: A random sample of experiences. The International Journal of the Addictions, 19, 367–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1982). Time alone in daily eiperience: loneliness or renewal? In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness; A sourcebook of research and theory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeFevre, J., Hendricks, C., Church, R., & McClintock, M. (1985). Psycho-logical and social behavior in couples over menstrual cycle: ‘On-the-spot’ sampling from everyday life. Paper presented at the 6th conference of the society for menstrual cycle research, Galveston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, R. J., Hamilton, J., Magna, S. et al. (1987). Experiential sampling in the study of multiple personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 19–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddi, S. R., Kobasa, S. C., & Hoover, M. (1979). An alienation test. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 19, 73–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnusson, D., & Endler, N. S. (1977). Interactional psychology: Present status and future prospects. In D. Magnusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology. Hillside: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margraf, J., Taylor, C. B., Ehlers, A. et al. (1987). Panic attacks in the natural environment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 558–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D., & Constantian, C. A. (1983). Intimacy and affiliation motives in daily living: An experience sampling analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 851–861.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1981). A cognitive-social learning approach to assess-ment. In T. Merluzzi, C. Glass M. Genest (Eds.), Cognitive assessment. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munroe, R. H., & Munroe, R. L. (1971a). Household density and infant care in an East African society. Journal of Social Psychology, 83, 9–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munroe, R. H., & Munroe, R. L. (1971b). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability in an East African society. Journal of Social Psychology, 83, 15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pawlik, K., & Buse, L. (1982). Rechnergestutzt verhattensregistrierung im Fetd: Beschreibung und erst psychometrische Uberprufung einfer neuen Erhebungsmethode. Zeitschrift fur Differentielle und Diagnostiche Psychobgie, 3, 101–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pervin, L. A. (1985). Personality: Current controversies, issues, and directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 83–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. (1977). How Americans use time. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1978). Spot observation: An introduction and examination. Quarterly Newsletter of the Institute for Comparative Human Development, 2, 21–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savin-Williams, R. C., & Demo, D. H. (1983). Situational and transnational determinants of adolescents’ self-feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 824–833.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorokin, P., & Berger, C. (1939). Time budgets and human behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, C. (1971). Random time sampling with self-observation for library cost studies: Unit costs of interlibrary loans and photocopies at a regional medical library. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 32, 153–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szalai, A. (1972). The use of time. Mouton: The Hauge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1937) How we spend our time and what we spend it for. Scientific Monthly, 44(5), 464–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Vallacher, R. R. (1977). Implicit psychology an introduction to social cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarmey, D. (1979). The psychology of eyewitness testimony. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuzanek, J. (1980). Work and leisure in the Soviet Union: A time budget analysis. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi .

Appendix: Experience-Sampling Form

Appendix: Experience-Sampling Form

Date:________Time Beeped:__________am/pm Time Filled Out__am/pm

As you were beeped…

What were you thinking about?___________________________________________________________

Where were you?______________________________________________________________________

What was the MAIN thing you were doing?_________________________________________________

What other things were you doing?________________________________________________________

WHY were you doing this particular activity?

(□) I had to (□) I wanted to do it (□) I had nothing else to do

 

Not at all

  

Some what

  

Quite

  

Very

How well were you concentrating?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Was it hard to concentrate?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

How self-conscious were you?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Did you feel good about yourself?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Were you in control of the situation?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Were you living up to your own expectations?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Were you living up to expectations of others?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

_______________________________________________________________________________________

Describe your mood as you were beeped:

 

Very

Quite

Some

Neither

Some

Quite

Very

 

Alert

0

o

.

.

o

0

Drowsy

Happy

0

o

.

.

o

0

Sad

Irritable

0

o

.

.

o

0

Cheerful

Strong

0

o

.

.

o

0

Weak

Active

0

o

.

.

o

0

Passive

Lonely

0

o

.

.

o

0

Sociable

Ashamed

0

o

.

.

o

0

Proud

Involved

0

o

.

.

o

0

Detached

Excited

0

o

.

.

o

0

Bored

Closed

0

o

.

.

o

0

Open

Clear

0

o

.

.

o

0

Confused

Tense

0

o

.

.

o

0

Relaxed

Competitive

0

o

.

.

o

0

Cooperative

Did you feel any physical discomfort as you were beeped:

Overall pain or discomfort

none

slight

bothersome

severe

 

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

 

Please specify:________________________________________________________

Who were you with?

(□) Alone

(□) Friend(s)

How many?

(□) Mother

 

___________________________________

 

Female (□)

Male (□)

(□) Father

(□) Strangers

 

(□) Sister(s) or brother(s)

(□) Other____________

 

_________________________________________________________________________________

Indicate how you felt about your activity:

 

low

 

high

Challenges of the activity

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Your skills in the activity

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

 

not at all

 

very much

Was this activity important to you?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Was this activity important to others?

0

1

2

3

4

6

6

7

8

9

Were you succeeding at what you were doing?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Do you wish you had been doing something else?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Were you satisfied with how you were doing?

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

How important was this activity in relation to your overall goals

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

If you had a choice

Who would you be with? ________________________________

What would you be doing?_______________________________

____________________________________________________________________________

Since you were last beeped has anything happened or have you done anything which could have affected the way you feel?

Nasty cracks, comments, etc. *****************

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R. (2014). Validity and Reliability of the Experience-Sampling Method. In: Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics