Skip to main content

Abstract Concepts and Metacognition: Searching for Meaning in Self and Others

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

Use of abstract concepts (e.g., truth) is one of the most sophisticated abilities that humans possess. Explaining how we develop this ability and how abstract concepts are represented constitutes one of the main challenges faced by theories of embodied and grounded cognition. In this chapter, we address this issue by focusing on the mechanisms underlying the processing of abstract concepts. We propose that metacognition—the set of capacities through which an operating subsystem is evaluated and represented by another subsystem—can ground the meaning of concepts, and that this grounding is particularly important for abstract concepts. In addition, metacognition can be applied to concept use itself. In this connection, the monitoring component of metacognition is particularly relevant: it can provide awareness of the inadequacies of our knowledge of abstract concepts, expressing a judgment of scarce confidence. This monitoring process can lead to two different but not mutually exclusive outcomes. We propose that both these outcomes have an embodied counterpart, the activation of the mouth motor system. The first is the use of inner speech, which aims to search for possible further meanings and/or to further clarify the word meaning. The second is the preparation to request the help of other—better if authoritative—people (social metacognition): when our knowledge has gaps, the need of social deference is stronger.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   299.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Acheson, D. J., Hamidi, M., Binder, J. R., & Postle, B. R. (2011). A common neural substrate for language production and verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 1358–1367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alderson-Day, B., & Fernyhough, C. (2015). Inner speech: Development, cognitive functions, phenomenology, and neurobiology. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 931.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116, 953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A., Gasser, B., & Barrès, V. (2014). Language is handy but is it embodied? Neuropsychologia, 55, 57–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldo, J. V., Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D., Ludy, C., Raskin, P., & Kim, J. (2005). Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain and Language, 92, 240–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Mazzuca, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2017). Pacifier overuse and conceptual relations of abstract and emotional concepts. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Mazzuca, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2020). Overusing the pacifier during infancy sets a footprint on abstract words processing. Journal of Child Language.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baronchelli, A., Gong, T., Puglisi, A., & Loreto, V. (2010). Modeling the emergence of universality in color naming patterns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 2403–2407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., Dutriaux, L., & Scheepers, C. (2018). Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 905–917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019). Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 120–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., & Tummolini, L. (2018). Abstract concepts, language and sociality: From acquisition to inner speech. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words as social tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: A study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2020). Touch me if you can: The intangible but grounded nature of abstract concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, commentary to Gilead, Trope & Liberman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Zarcone, E. (2016). Grounding abstractness: Abstract concepts and the activation of the mouth. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1498.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brenna, B. A. (1995). The metacognitive reading strategies of five early readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 53–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2009). How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 121–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: The forgotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constable, M. D., Kritikos, A., & Bayliss, A. P. (2011). Grasping the concept of personal property. Cognition, 119, 430–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, G. (2014). Thinking in words: Language as an embodied medium of thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 371–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, G. (2018). Language as a disruptive technology: Abstract concepts, embodiment and the flexible mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, G. (2019). More than a scaffold: Language is a neuroenhancement. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyer, F. R., & Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Abstract semantics in the motor system?—An event-related fMRI study on passive reading of semantic word categories carrying abstract emotional and mental meaning. Cortex, 100, 52–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falandays, J. B., & Spivey, M. J. (2019). Abstract meanings may be more dynamic, due to their sociality: Comment on” Words as social tools “ by Borghi et al. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 175–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fini, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Sociality to reach objects and to catch meaning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 127–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D. (2012). The role of metacognition in human social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 367, 2213–2223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furby, L. (1980). The origins and early development of possessive behavior. Political Psychology, 30–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., Roepstorff, A., & Tylén, K. (2012). Coming to terms: An experimental quantification of the coordinative benefits of linguistic interaction. Psychological Science, 23, 931–939.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghio, M., Vaghi, M. M. S., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). Fine-grained semantic categorization across the abstract and concrete domains. PLoS One, 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (2019). Turning social tools into tools for action. Comment on Words as social tools by Borghi et al. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 172–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P. Y., Lopes, M., & Baranes, A. (2013). Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: Computational and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 585–593.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Granito, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2015). Naming a lego world. The role of language in the acquisition of abstract concepts. PLoS One, 10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A. C. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of Metacognition in education. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344, 1243091.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holton, D., & Clarke, D. (2006). Scaffolding and metacognition. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 37, 127–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261–1277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kominsky, J. F., Langthorne, P., & Keil, F. C. (2016). The better part of not knowing: Virtuous ignorance. Developmental Psychology, 52, 31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kominsky, J. F., & Keil, F. C. (2014). Overestimation of knowledge about word meanings: The “misplaced meaning” effect. Cognitive Science, 38, 1604–1633.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kominsky, J. F., Zamm, A. P., & Keil, F. C. (2018). Knowing when help is needed: A developing sense of causal complexity. Cognitive Science, 42, 491–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Dean, D., Jr. (2004). Metacognition: A bridge between cognitive psychology and educational practice. Theory into Practice, 43, 268–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (2009). Investigations in cognitive grammar (Vol. 42). Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lees, J., & Cikara, M. (2019). Inaccurate group meta-perceptions drive negative out-group attributions in competitive contexts. Nature Human Behaviour, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langland-Hassan, P., & Vicente, A. (2018). Inner Speech: New Voices. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lidstone, J. S., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2010). The roles of private speech and inner speech in planning during middle childhood: Evidence from a dual task paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 438–451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lupyan, G., & Winter, B. (2018). Language is more abstract than you think, or, why aren’t languages more iconic? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mannella, F., Santucci, V. G., Somogyi, E., Jacquey, L., O’Regan, K. J., & Baldassarre, G. (2018). Know your body through intrinsic goals. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 12, 30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez, M. E. (2006). What is metacognition? Phi Delta Kappan, 87, 696–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzuca, C., Lugli, L., Benassi, M., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A. M. (2018). Abstract, emotional and concrete concepts and the activation of mouth-hand effectors. PeerJ, 6, e5987.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, P. I., Campbell, C., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2012). Effects of emotional and sensorimotor knowledge in semantic processing of concrete and abstract nouns. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 275.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach (Vol. 9). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 329–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ponari, M., Norbury, C. F., & Vigliocco, G. (2018). Acquisition of abstract concepts is influenced by emotional valence. Developmental Science, 21, e12549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2012). Beyond Human Nature: How Culture and Experience Shape Our Lives. Penguin UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proust, J. (2012). Metacognition and mindreading: one or two functions. Foundations of Metacognition, 234–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of ‘meaning’. Philosophical Papers, 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabb, N., Fernbach, P. M., & Sloman, S. A. (2019). Individual representation in a community of knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 351–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Akin, C., & Luh, W. M. (1992). Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition, 20, 96–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scorolli, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2018). Cues of control modulate the ascription of object ownership. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 82, 929–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, N. (2012). Reward prediction error signals are meta-representational. Nous, 48, 314–341.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., & Frith, C. D. (2014). Supra-personal cognitive control and metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 186–193.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, N. (2018). Metacognition and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, N. (2019). Concept-metacognition. Mind & Language.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siakaluk, P. D., Pexman, P. M., Aguilera, L., Owen, W. J., & Sears, C. R. (2008). Evidence for the activation of sensorimotor information during visual word recognition: The body–object interaction effect. Cognition, 106, 433–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topolinski, S., Maschmann, I. T., Pecher, D., & Winkielman, P. (2014). Oral approach–avoidance: Affective consequences of muscular articulation dynamics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009). Motormouth: Mere exposure depends on stimulus-specific motor simulations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 423.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S. T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 1767–1777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1934). 1986). Thought and Language (rev). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M. T., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Varieties of abstract concepts and their multiple dimensions. Language and Cognition, 11, 403–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M. T., Nicoletti, R. & Borghi, A. M. (under review). Sensorimotor and Interoceptive dimensions in concrete and abstract concepts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang J., Conder J. A., Blitzer D.N., Shinkareva S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 1459–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Metaanalysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. H. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. P., & Atkins, J. G. (2009). The role of metacognition in teaching reading comprehension to primary students. In Handbook of Metacognition in Education (pp. 38–56). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., & Summerfield, C. (2012). Metacognition in human decision making: Confidence and error monitoring. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b., 367, 1310–1321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zannino, G., Fini, C., Benassi, M., Carlesimo, A. & Borghi, A. M. (submitted). Articulatory suppression delays processing of abstract concepts: the role of inner speech.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna M. Borghi .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Borghi, A.M., Fini, C., Tummolini, L. (2021). Abstract Concepts and Metacognition: Searching for Meaning in Self and Others. In: Robinson, M.D., Thomas, L.E. (eds) Handbook of Embodied Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-78471-3_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics