Skip to main content

Executive Functioning Theory and Autism

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

The triad of characteristics that defines the autistic disorder includes the following: social and communication impairments, and restricted, stereotypical patterns of behavior and interests (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994, 2000, for all symptoms see Table 8.1). There are different classic autism-like conditions, and these other pervasive developmental disorders (PDD), such as Asperger syndrome and PDD not otherwise specified (PDDNOS), are part of the broader phenotype of autism. In the current classification system, DSM-IV (APA, 1994, 2000), also Rett syndrome and the Disintegration disorder are considered autism-like conditions. However, in the current chapter we will focus solely on classic autism, Asperger syndrome, and PDDNOS. The combination of these three disorders is referred to as an autism spectrum disorder (ASD), which is the term we will use throughout this chapter.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   349.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   449.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   449.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2009). Inhibition and the validity of the Stroop task for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1112–1121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inhibition in autism: Children with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1052–1063.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Agam, Y., Huang, J., & Sekuler, R. (2010). Neural correlates of sequence encoding in visuomotor learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(3), 1418–1424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Agam, Y., Joseph, R. M., Barton, J. J., & Manoach, D. S. (2010). Reduced cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 52(1), 336–347.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(3), 137–145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambery, F. Z., Russell, A. J., Perry, K., Morris, R., & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10(6), 551–564.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anckarsater, H., Stahlberg, O., Larson, T., Hakansson, C., Jutblad, S. B., Niklasson, L., et al. (2006). The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(7), 1239–1244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: The Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barcelo, F. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroreport, 10(6), 1299–1303.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnard, L., Muldoon, K., Hasan, R., O’Brien, G., & Stewart, M. (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism, 12(2), 125–141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (p. 312). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S. J., Hanson, C. A., Puffenberger, S. S., Benninger, K. L., & Benninger, W. B. (2010). A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39(6), 825–836.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belmonte, M. K., & Yurgelun Todd, D. A. (2003). Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 651–664.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67(4), 1816–1835.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D. B., Krikorian, R., & Huha, E. M. (1998). The Corsi block-tapping task: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain and Cognition, 38(3), 317–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. The Journal of General Psychology, 39, 15–22.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, H. J. C., Aerts, F. H. T. M., Spaendonck, K. P. M. V., Cools, A. R., & Teunisse, J.-P. (2003). Central coherence and cognitive shifting in relation to social improvement in high-functioning young adults with autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(4), 502–511.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D., et al. (2003). Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition. NeuroImage, 20(2), 737–751.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., & Happé, F. (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and executive dysfunction: Planning drawing in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 387–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., Cowell, P., Howard, M., Broks, P., Farrant, A., Roberts, N., et al. (2005). A combined clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroanatomical study of adults with high functioning autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(3), 165–213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bramham, J., Ambery, F., Young, S., Morris, R., Russell, A., Xenitidis, K., et al. (2009). Executive functioning differences between adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autistic spectrum disorder in initiation, planning and strategy formation. Autism, 13(3), 245–264.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brugha, T. S., Mcmanus, S., Bankart, J., Scott, F., Purdon, S., Smith, J., et al. (2011). Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community in England. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(5), 459–465.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cambridge. (2002). CANTAB. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105–1122.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Marsh, W. L., Hamburger, S. D., Schubert, A. B., et al. (1997). Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(3), 374–383.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Causton-Theoharis, J., Ashby, C., & Cosier, M. (2009). Islands of loneliness: Exploring social interaction through the autobiographies of individuals with autism. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 47(2), 84–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, A. S., Han, Y. M. Y., Sze, S. L., Cheung, M.-C., Leung, W. W.-M., Chan, R. C. K., et al. (2011). Disordered connectivity associated with memory deficits in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 237–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., & Baron Cohen, S. (1992). Understanding drawings and beliefs: A further test of the metarepresentation theory of autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(6), 1105–1112.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christ, S. E., Holt, D. D., White, D. A., & Green, L. (2007). Inhibitory control in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1155–1165.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christ, S. E., Kester, L. E., Bodner, K. E., & Miles, J. H. (2011). Evidence for selective inhibitory control impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 25(6), 690–701.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christ, S. E., White, D. A., Mandernach, T., & Keys, B. A. (2001). Inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(3), 653–669.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, B. A., Constantine, L. J., Hendren, R., Rocke, D., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166(2–3), 210–222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain [Dissertation]. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34(2), 819B.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: Local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 225–230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cui, J., Gao, D., Chen, Y., Zou, X., & Wang, Y. (2010). Working memory in early-school-age children with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 958–967.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35(12), 777–786.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, M., Prins, P., Schmand, B., & Geurts, H. M. (2011). Executieve functietraining met een computergame bij kinderen met ASS. Paper presented at the Even-Waardig: Autisme, praktijk en onderzoek.

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Martino, A., Ross, K., Uddin, L. Q., Sklar, A. B., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2009). Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 63–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dichter, G., Radonovich, K., Turner-Brown, L., Lam, K., Holtzclaw, T., & Bodfish, J. (2010). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the Dimension-Change Card Sort Task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 448–456.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eskes, G., Bryson, S., & Mccormick, T. (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 61–73.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eslinger, P. J., & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology, 35(12), 1731–1741.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, N., & Happé, F. (2005). A training study of theory of mind and executive function in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 757–771.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, E., O’Malley, C., & Wood, D. (2004). A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. Developmental Science, 7(1), 103–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (2005). Epidemiology of autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(Suppl. 10), 3–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(1), 101–135.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). A new look at language and communication in autism. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24(2), 123–150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1–3), 115–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14(10), 433–438.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., Brooks, P. J., & Samuels, M. C. (1996). Inference and action in early causal reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 120–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabig, C. S. (2008). Verbal working memory and story retelling in school-age children with autism. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39(4), 498–511.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ganz, J. B., Davis, J. L., Lund, E. M., Goodwyn, F. D., & Simpson, R. L. (2011). Meta-analysis of PECS with individuals with ASD: Investigation of targeted versus non-targeted outcomes, participant characteristics, and implementation phase. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 406–418.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Practitioner review: Short-term and working memory impairments in neurodevelopmental disorders: Diagnosis and remedial support. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(1), 4–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age [Article]. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177–190.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiades, S., Szatmari, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Duku, E., Bryson, S., Roberts, W., et al. (2007). Structure of the autism symptom phenotype: A proposed multidimensional model. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 46(2), 188–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Begeer, S., & Stockmann, L. (2009). Brief report: Inhibitory control of socially relevant stimuli in children with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(11), 1603–1607.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H.M., & Bringmann, L. (2010). Planning in autisme: Een kwalitatieve en kwantitatieve analyse. Wetenschappelijk Tijdschrift Autisme, 1, 4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(2), 74–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H.M. & De Wit (in press). Goal-directed action control in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. No update on Geurts, Sinzig, Booth & Happé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., & Jansen, M. D. (2012). Short report: A retrospective chart study: The pathway to a diagnosis for adults referred for ASD assessment. Autism, 16, 299–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Luman, M., & Meel, C. S. V. (2008). What’s in a game: The effect of social motivation on interference control in boys with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 848–857.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Sinzig, J., Booth, R., & Happé, F. (submitted). Neuropsychological heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: A comparison with ADHD. Autism Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 836–854.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., & Vissers, M. E. (2012). Elderly with autism: Executive functions and memory. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 665–675.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gezondheidsraad. (2009). Autismespectrumstoornissen: Een leven lang anders. Den Haag: Gezondheidsraad.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S. J., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2008). Atypical recruitment of medial prefrontal cortex in autism spectrum disorders: An fMRI study of two executive function tasks. Neuropsychologia, 46(9), 2281–2291.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M., Astor, B. C., Denckla, M. B., et al. (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 279–293.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J. (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 433–440.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gomarus, H. K., Wijers, A. A., Minderaa, R. B., & Althaus, M. (2009). ERP correlates of selective attention and working memory capacities in children with ADHD and/or PDD-NOS. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(1), 60–72.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griebling, J., Minshew, N. J., Bodner, K., Libove, R., Bansal, R., Konasale, P., et al. (2010). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex magnetic resonance imaging measurements and cognitive performance in autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 25(7), 856–863.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70(4), 817–832.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hala, S., Hug, S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Executive function and false-belief understanding in preschool children: Two tasks are harder than one. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4(3), 275–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216–222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25–39.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Ronald, A. (2008). The fractionable autism triad: A review of evidence from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychology Review, 18(4), 287–304.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218–1220.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, H., Schwartz, C., Mundy, P., Burnette, C., Sutton, S., Zahka, N., et al. (2006). Response monitoring, the error-related negativity, and differences in social behavior in autism. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 96–109.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24(2), 189–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44(14), 2822–2835.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofvander, B., Delorme, R., Chaste, P., Nyden, A., Wentz, E., Stahlberg, O., et al. (2009). Psychiatric and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry, 9, 35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., Place, M., Dunning, D. L., Hilton, K. A., & Elliott, J. G. (2009). Working memory deficits can be overcome: Impacts of training and medication on working memory in children with ADHD. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(6), 827–836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1447–1459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 498–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477–492.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Daibhis, A., et al. (2007). Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2234–2245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, K., Madden, A. K., Bramham, J., & Russell, A. J. (2011). Response inhibition in adults with autism spectrum disorder compared to attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(7), 903–912.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R. M., Steele, S. D., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: Failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory? Neuropsychologia, 43(10), 1400–1411.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., Kana, R. K., & Minshew, N. J. (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 951–961.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127(Pt 8), 1811–1821.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M., & Varma, S. (2007). The organization of thinking: What functional brain imaging reveals about the neuroarchitecture of complex cognition. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(3), 153–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2006). Sentence comprehension in autism: Thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain, 129(Pt 9), 2484–2493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2009). Atypical frontal-posterior synchronization of theory of mind regions in autism during mental state attribution. Social Neuroscience, 4(2), 135–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2007). Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: Decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biological Psychiatry, 62(3), 198–206.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keary, C. J., Minshew, N. J., Bansal, R., Goradia, D., Fedorov, S., Keshavan, M. S., et al. (2009). Corpus callosum volume and neurocognition in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(6), 834–841.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kilincaslan, A., Mukaddes, N. M., Kucukyazici, G. S., & Gurvit, H. (2010). Assessment of executive/attentional performance in Asperger’s disorder. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 21(4), 289–299.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinhans, N. M., Richards, T., Sterling, L., Stegbauer, K. C., Mahurin, R., Johnson, L. C., et al. (2008). Abnormal functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders during face processing. Brain, 131(Pt 4), 1000–1012.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(6), 781–791.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koshino, H., Carpenter, P. A., Minshew, N. J., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Just, M. A. (2005). Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. NeuroImage, 24(3), 810–821.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koshino, H., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2008). fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: Visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cerebral Cortex, 18(2), 289–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R., & Goldberg, M. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 557–573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langen, M., Durston, S., Kas, M. J., Van Engeland, H., & Staal, W. G. (2011). The neurobiology of repetitive behavior: …and men. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 356–365.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langen, M., Leemans, A., Johnston, P., Ecker, C., Daly, E., Murphy, C. M., et al. (2012). Fronto-striatal circuitry and inhibitory control in autism: Findings from diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cortex, 48, 183–193.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P. S., Yerys, B. E., Della Rosa, A., Foss-Feig, J., Barnes, K. A., James, J. D., et al. (2009). Functional connectivity of the inferior frontal cortex changes with age in children with autism spectrum disorders: A fcMRI study of response inhibition. Cerebral Cortex, 19(8), 1787–1794.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemon, J. M., Gargaro, B., Enticott, P. G., & Rinehart, N. J. (2011). Brief report: Executive functioning in autism spectrum disorders: A gender comparison of response inhibition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(3), 352–356.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leyfer, O. T., Folstein, S. E., Bacalman, S., Davis, N. O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., et al. (2006). Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: Interview development and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(7), 849–861.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Morris, R., et al. (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 261–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. In D. Dagenbach (Ed.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (Vol. 14). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(4), 445–460.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61(4), 474–481.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, K. E., Lazar, N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Eddy, W. F., et al. (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology, 59(6), 834–840.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macleod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maes, J. H., Eling, P. A., Wezenberg, E., Vissers, C. T., & Kan, C. C. (2011). Attentional set shifting in autism spectrum disorder: Differentiating between the role of perseveration, learned irrelevance, and novelty processing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(2), 210–217.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahone, E. M., Powell, S. K., Loftis, C. W., Goldberg, M. C., Denckla, M. B., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor persistence and inhibition in autism and ADHD. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(5), 622–631.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & Prior, M. (2003). The reach-to-grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 393–403.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, R. A., Williams, D. L., Kana, R. K., Minshew, N., & Just, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind disruption and recruitment of the right hemisphere during narrative comprehension in autism. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 269–280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matson, J. L., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2008). The token economy for children with intellectual disability and/or autism: A review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30(2), 240–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. S. (2007). Comorbid psychopathology with autism spectrum disorder in children: An overview. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(4), 341–352.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, R. G., & Damasio, A. R. (1982). Childhood autism from the point of view of behavioral neurology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12(2), 195–205.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mcalonan, G. M., Cheung, V., Cheung, C., Suckling, J., Lam, G. Y., Tai, K. S., et al. (2005). Mapping the brain in autism. A voxel-based MRI study of volumetric differences and intercorrelations in autism. Brain, 128(Pt 2), 268–276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mckinnon, M. C., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: Theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition, 102(2), 179–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2001). The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(8), 1095–1101.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., & Payton, J. B. (1992). Neuropsychological functioning nonmentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 749–761.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16(3), 327–334.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mosconi, M. W., Kay, M., D’Cruz, A. M., Seidenfeld, A., Guter, S., Stanford, L. D., et al. (2009). Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine, 39(9), 1559–1566.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, U., Liebermann-Finestone, D. P., Carpendale, J. I., Hammond, S. I., & Bibok, M. B. (2012). Knowing minds, controlling actions: The developmental relations between theory of mind and executive function from 2 to 4 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(2), 331–348.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nydén, A., Niklasson, L., Stahlberg, O., Anckarsater, H., Wentz, E., Rastam, M., et al. (2010). Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological aspects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 1659–1668.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9(4), 491–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R., Klin, A., et al. (2004). Performance on Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from the collaborative programs of excellence in autism network. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 139–150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 171–177.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Mcevoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6(3), 415–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(4), 415–433.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1081–1105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(1), 59–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 257–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., Mcmahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: An information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(6), 1015–1032.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 974–990.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E. (2010a). The development of core cognitive skills in autism: A 3-year prospective study. Child Development, 81(5), 1400–1416.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E. (2010b). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 530–544.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 337–344.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2000). Theory of mind and executive function: Is there a developmental relationship? In D. J. Cohen, S. Baron Cohen, & H. Tager Flusberg (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 150–181). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poljac, E., Simon, S., Ringlever, L., Kalcik, D., Groen, W. B., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2009). Impaired task switching performance in children with dyslexia but not in children with autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(2), 401–416.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poustka, L., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Henze, R., Vomstein, K., Haffner, J., & Sieltjes, B. (2012). Fronto-temporal disconnectivity and symptom severity in children with autism spectrum disorder. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 13(4), 269–280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Brief report: Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(4), 581–590.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I., & Tuchman, R. F. (2008). Autism: Definition, neurobiology, screening, diagnosis. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 55(5), 1129–1146. viii.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raymaekers, R., Antrop, I., Van Der Meere, J. J., Wiersema, J. R., & Roeyers, H. (2007). HFA and ADHD: A direct comparison on state regulation and response inhibition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(4), 418–427.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raymaekers, R., Van Der Meere, J., & Roeyers, H. (2004). Event-rate manipulation and its effect on arousal modulation and response inhibition in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 74–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raymaekers, R., Van Der Meere, J., & Roeyers, H. (2006). Response inhibition and immediate arousal in children with high-functioning autism. Child Neuropsychology, 12(4–5), 349–359.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, P., & Mccarthy, J. (2012). Cross-modal attention-switching is impaired in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 947–953.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, P., Watts, H., & Truzoli, R. (2013). Flexibility in young people with autism spectrum disorders on a card sort task. Autism, 17, 162–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. (2009). Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Cognition, 71, 362–368.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rommelse, N. J., Franke, B., Geurts, H. M., Hartman, C. A., & Buitelaar, J. (2010). Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(3), 281–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, J. M. (1985). Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(1), 23–36.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 103–112.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 77–86.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 74–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schipul, S. E., Williams, D. L., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2012). Distinctive neural processes during learning in autism. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 937–950.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, S., & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 59(1), 7–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Butcher, B. (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 1017–1027.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130(1–2), 3–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafritz, K. M., Dichter, G. S., Baranek, G. T., & Belger, A. (2008). The neural circuitry mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 63(10), 974–980.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921–929.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinzig, J., Morsch, D., Bruning, N., Schmidt, M., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Inhibition, flexibility, working memory and planning in autism spectrum disorders with and without comorbid ADHD-symptoms. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 2(1), 4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M., Ozonoff, S. J., Cummings, N., & Carter, C. S. (2008). Cognitive control in autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 26(2), 239–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M., Ozonoff, S. J., Ursu, S., Ravizza, S., Cummings, N., Ly, S., et al. (2009). The neural substrates of cognitive control deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47(12), 2515–2526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, L., & Pry, R. (2002). Joint attention and set-shifting in young children with autism. Autism, 6(4), 383–396.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanfield, A. C., Mcintosh, A. M., Spencer, M. D., Philip, R., Gaur, S., & Lawrie, S. M. (2008). Towards a neuroanatomy of autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. European Psychiatry, 23(4), 289–299.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, S. D., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Spatial working memory deficits in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 605–612.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G., & López, B. (2010). Task-switching abilities in children with autism spectrum disorder. The European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8(2), 244–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sumiyoshi, C., Kawakubo, Y., Suga, M., Sumiyoshi, T., & Kasai, K. (2011). Impaired ability to organize information in individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their siblings. Neuroscience Research, 69(3), 252–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1390–1401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verté, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2006a). Executive functioning in children with an autism spectrum disorder: Can we differentiate within the spectrum? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 351–372.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verté, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2006b). The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response variability in child psychopathology. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 151(1), 5–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vissers, M. E., Cohen, M., & Geurts, H. M. (2012). Brain connectivity and high functioning autism: A promising path of research that needs refined models, methodological convergence, and stronger behavioral links. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 604–625.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 135–170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wass, S. (2011). Distortions and disconnections: Disrupted brain connectivity in autism. Brain and Cognition, 75(1), 18–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welchew, D. E., Ashwin, C., Berkouk, K., Salvador, R., Suckling, J., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2005). Functional disconnectivity of the medial temporal lobe in Asperger’s syndrome. Biological Psychiatry, 57(9), 991–998.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M. C., Satterlee-Cartmell, T., & Stine, M. (1999). Towers of Hanoi and London: Contribution of working memory and inhibition to performance. Brain and Cognition, 41(2), 231–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Training attention-switching ability in adults with AD/HD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 44–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, S. J., Burgess, P. W., & Hill, E. L. (2009). Impairments on “open-ended” executive function tests in autism. Autism Research, 2(3), 138–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J. O., Maybery, M. T., & Durkin, K. (2006). Inner speech impairments in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(8), 857–865.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Nigg, J. T., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 24, 195–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D., Goldstein, G., Carpenter, P., & Minshew, N. (2005). Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 747–756.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 21–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yerys, B. E., Hepburn, S. L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). Executive function in preschoolers with autism: Evidence consistent with a secondary deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1068–1079.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Harrison, B., Celano, M. J., Giedd, J. N., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13(5), 523–538.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Jacques, S., Burack, J. A., & Frye, D. (2002). The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 171–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zinke, K., Fries, E., Altgassen, M., Kirschbaum, C., Dettenborn, L., & Kliegel, M. (2010). Visuospatial short-term memory explains deficits in tower task planning in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 16(3), 229–241.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hilde M. Geurts .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Geurts, H.M., de Vries, M., van den Bergh, S.F.W.M. (2014). Executive Functioning Theory and Autism. In: Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J. (eds) Handbook of Executive Functioning. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics