Skip to main content

Environmental Perception and Cognition

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Advances in Environment, Behavior, and Design ((AEBD,volume 2))

Abstract

Environment and behavior research investigates the mutual relations between the sociophysical environment at all scales and human behavior at all levels of analysis (Moore, 1987). It is generally assumed that there are an “actor,” an external environment or situation, an input from situation to actor resulting in a “psychological response,” and, on the basis of this response, an output from actor to situation termed action (Golledge, 1987). In studies of environmental perception and cognition, the existence of motives, goals, and attitudes toward action alternatives are usually taken for granted, and the psychological responses or processes mediating between the environment and actions are made a primary focus. These processes include the picking up of information from, and about, the environment; the internal, perceptual, and cognitive representation of the information; and judgments, decisions, and choices made on the basis of the represented information. Knowledge gained about perceptual-cognitive processes may improve the quality of human environments through policy, planning, and design, to the extent that it tells how to plan and design environments that do not interfere with the proper functioning of these processes. Examples are the work of Appleyard (1976) and Lynch (1960), resulting in suggested guidelines for how to make cities legible.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Acredolo, L. (1985). Coordinating perspectives on infant spatial orientation. In R. Cohen (Ed.), The development of spatial cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo, L. P., Adams, A., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1984). The role of self-produced movement and visual tracking in infant spatial orientation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 312–327.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. L. (1981). A developmental perspective on the effects of "subdividing" mac-rospatial experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 120–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. L., & Kirasic, K. C. (1985). Effects of the cognitive organization of route knowledge on judgments of macrospatial distance. Memory & Cognition, 13, 218–227.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Appleyard, D. (1976). Planning a pluralistic city. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aragonés, J. I., & Arrendondo, J. M. (1985). Structure of urban cognitive maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 197–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axia, G., Baroni, M. R., & Mainard-Peron, E. (1986). Children’s and adults’ memory for places. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, J. C., Wagner, M., and Noma, E. (1982). Impossible cognitive spaces. Geographical Analysis, 14, 204–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartram, D. J. (1980). Comprehending spatial information: The relative efficiency of different methods of presenting information about bus routes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 103–110.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Biel, A. (1983). Children’s spatial representation of their neighbourhood: A step towards a general spatial competence. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2, 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Book, A. (1986a). Human recognition memory for nonverbal visual information. Unpublished manuscript, University of Umeá, Department of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Böök, A. (1986b). Memory for scene perspectives and spatial orientation. Unpublished manuscript, University of Umeá, Department of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Böök, A., & Garling, T. (1981). Maintenance of orientation during locomotion in unfamiliar environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 995–1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bronzaft, A. L., Dobrow, S. B., & O’Hanlon, T. J. (1976). Spatial orientation in a subway system. Environment and Behavior, 8, 575–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadwallader, M. (1975). A behavioral model of consumer spatial decision making. Economic Geography, 51, 339–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cadwallader, M. (1981). Towards a cognitive gravity model: The case of consumer spatial behaviour. Regional Studies, 15, 275–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canter, D. (1977). The psychology of place. London: Architectural Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canter, D. (1983). The purposive evaluation of places: A facet approach. Environment and Behavior, 15, 659–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canter, D., & Rees, K. (1982). A multivariate model of housing satisfaction. International Review of Applied Psychology, 31, 185–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canter, D., & Tagg, S. K. (1975). Distance estimation in cities. Environment and Behavior, 7, 59–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canter, D., Brown, J., & Groat, L. (1985). A multiple sorting procedure for studying conceptual systems. In M. Brenner, J. Brown, & D. Canter (Eds.), The research interview. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J., & Knight, R. J. (1985). The reliability of ratings of familiarity of environmental stimuli: A generalizability analysis. Environment and Behavior, 17, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conning, A. M., & Byrne (1984). Pointing to preschool children’s spatial competence: A study in natural settings. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 4, 165–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. H., & Appleyard, D. (1980). Streets of San Francisco: Brunswik’s lens model applied to urban inference and assessment. Journal of Social Issues, 36, 72–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. H., & Feimar, N. R. (1987). Environmental assessment. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, S., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1985). Developmental changes in neighborhood scene recognition. Children’s Environments Quarterly, 2, 38–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. (1970). The cognitive structure of an urban shopping centre. Environment and Behavior, 2, 13–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. M. (1981). Maps and mappings as metaphors for spatial representation. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, & N. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behavior across the life span. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. M., & Stea, D. (1973). Cognitive maps and spatial behavior: Process and products. In R. M. Downs & D. Stea (Eds.), Image and environment. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W. (1980). Environmental cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 259–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., & Pezdek, K. (1980). Cognitive mapping: Knowledge of real-world distance and location information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 13–24.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., Marrero, D. G., & Butler, P. A. (1981). Environmental learning and cognitive mapping. Environment and Behavior, 13, 83–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., Skorpanich, M. A., Garling, T., Bryant, K. J., & Bresolin, B. (1984). The effects of pathway configuration, landmarks and stress on environmental cognition. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 4, 323–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foley, J. E., & Cohen, A. J. (1984). Working mental representations of the environment. Environment and Behavior, 16, 713–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frese, M., & Sabini, J. (1985). Action theory: An introduction. In M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), Goal directed behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garland, H. C., Haynes, J. J., & Grubb, G. C. (1979). Transit map color coding and street detail: Effects on trip planning performance. Environment and Behavior, 11, 162–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T. (1976). The structural analysis of environmental perception and cognition: A multidimensional scaling approach. Environment and Behavior, 8, 385–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Böök, A., Lindberg, E., & Nilsson, T. (1981). Memory for the spatial layout of the everyday physical environment: Factors affecting rate of acquisition. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Böök, A., & Ergezen, N. (1982). Memory for the spatial layout of the everyday physical environment: Differential rates of acquisition of different types of information. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 23, 23–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Lindberg, E., & Mantyla, T. (1983). Orientation in buildings: Effects of familiarity, visual access, and orientation aids. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 177–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1984). Cognitive mapping of large-scale environments: The interrelationship of action plans, acquisition, and orientation. Environment and Behavior, 16, 3–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986a). Spatial orientation and wayfinding in the designed environment: A conceptual analysis and some suggestions for postoccupan-cy evaluation. Journal of Architectural and Planning Research, 3, 55–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Lindberg, E., Carreiras, M., & Book, A. (1986b). Reference systems in cognitive maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genereux, R. L., Ward, L. M., & Russell, J. A. (1983). The behavioral component in the meaning of places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). The development of perception. In P. H. Mussen, Flavell, J. H., & Markman, E. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed.), Vol. 3. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton & Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton & Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraudo, M.-D., & Peruch, P. (1986). Spatiotemporal aspects of urban space representation. Unpublished.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G. (1976). Methods and methodological issues in environmental cognition research. In G. T. Moore & R. G. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental knowing. Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Huchinson & Ross.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G. (1981). Misconceptions, misinterpretations, and misrepresentations of behavioral approaches in human geography. Environment and Planning A, 13, 1325–1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G. (1987). Environmental cognition. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., & Hubert, L. J. (1982). Some comments on non-Euclidean mental maps. Environment and Planning A, 14, 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., & Spector, A. N. (1978). Comprehending the urban environment: Theory and practice. Geographical Analysis, 10, 403–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., Smith, T. R., Pellegrino, J. W., Doherty, S. E., & Marshall, S. P. (1985). A conceptual model and empirical analysis of children’s acquisition of spatial knowledge. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 125–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., Gale, N., & Richardson, G. D. (1986a). Cognitive maps of cities I: The nature and properties of cognitive maps. Unpublished.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., Gale, N., & Richardson, G. D. (1986b). Cognitive maps of cities II: Studies of selected populations. Unpublished.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groat, L. (1981). Meaning in architecture: New directions and sources. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1, 73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groat, L. (1982). Meaning in post-modern architecture: An examination using the multiple sorting task. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, R., Purcell, A. T., Thorne, R., & Metcalfe, J. (1976). Multidimensional scaling analysis of interior, designed spaces. Environment and Behavior, 8, 595–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halperin, W. C., Gale, N., & Gorenflo, L. J. (1988). Current issues in behavioral modeling of consumer choice. In R. G. Golledge & H. Timmermans (eds.), Behavioral modeling approaches in geography and planning. London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, S. (1980). Spatial diversification and multipurpose travel: Implications for choice theory. Geographical Analysis, 12, 245–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. A., & Moore, G. T. (1973). The development of spatial cognition. In R. Downs & D. Stea (Eds.), Image and environment. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hazen, N. L. (1982). Spatial exploration and spatial knowledge: Individual and developmental differences in very young children. Child Development, 49, 623–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazen, N. L., Lockman, J. J., & Pick, H. L., Jr. (1978). The development of children’s representations of large-scale environments. Child Development, 49, 623–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1981). An examination of the constructivist approach and Gibsonian approaches to environmental psychology. Population and Environment, 8, 227–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H., & Wohlwill, J. F. (1987). Environmental cognition in children. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman, J. F., Shiraki, J. H., & Miller, B. S. (1985). Young children’s ability to infer spatial relationships: Evidence from a large, familiar environment. Child Development, 56, 1195–1203.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, J. F., Norton, L. M., & Klein, C. A. (1986). Children’s distance estimates in a large-scale environment: A search for the route angularity effect. Environment and Behavior, 18, 533–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holohan, C. J. (1982). Environmental psychology. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holohan, C. J. (1986). Environmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 381–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, R. (1974). Images of the retailing environment: An example of the use of the repertory grid methodology. Environment and Behavior, 6, 470–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, M. E. (1985a). Enhancing a building’s imageability. Journal of Architectural and Planning Research, 2, 151–168.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, M. E. (1985b). Environmental learning without being there. Environment and Behavior, 16, 307–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ittelson, W. H. (1973). Environment perception and contemporary perceptual theory. In W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and cognition. New York: Seminar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kameron, J. (1973). Experimental studies of environment perception. In W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and cognition. New York: Seminar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S. (1983). A model of person-environment compatibility. Environment and Behavior, 15, 311–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1982). Cognition and environment. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasmar, J. V. (1970). The development of a usable lexicon of environmental descriptors. Environment and Behavior, 2, 153–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirasic, K. C., Allen, G. L., & Siegel, A. W. (1984). Expression of configurational knowledge of large-scale environments. Environment and Behavior, 16, 687–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuipers, B. (1978). Modeling spatial knowledge. Cognitive Science, 2, 129–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuipers, B. (1982). The "map in the head" metaphor. Environment and Behavior, 14, 202–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, R., Lachman, J. L., & Butterfield, E. C. (1979). Cognitive psychology and information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landwehr, K. (1988). Environmental perception: An ecological perspective. In D. Canter, M. Krampen, & D. Stea (Eds.), Environmental perspectives. Guildford: Gower.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leff, H. L. (1974). Experience, environment, and human potentials. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M., Jankovic, I. N., & Palij, M. (1982). Principles of spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M., Marchon, I., & Hanley, G. (1984). The placement and misplacement of you-are-here maps. Environment and Behavior, 16, 139–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindberg, E., & Gärling, T. (1982). Acquisition of locational information about reference points during locomotion: The role of central information processing. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 23, 207–218.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lindberg, E., & Gärling, T. (1983). Acquisition of different types of locational information in cognitive maps: Automatic or effortful processing? Psychological Research, 45,19–38.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, R. E. (1976). Cognition, preference, and behavior in space: An examination of the structural linkages. Economic Geography, 52, 241–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louviere, J. J., & Henley, D. A. (1977). Information integration theory applied to student apartment selection decisions. Geographical Analysis, 9, 130–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louviere, J. J., & Norman, K. (1977). Applications of information processing theory to the analysis of urban travel demand. Environment and Behavior, 9, 91–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenthal, D., & Riel, M. (1972). The nature of perceived and imagined environments. Environment and Behavior, 4, 189–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. B., & Olshavsky, R. W. (1975). Cognitive maps and spatial behavior of consumers. Geographical Analysis, 7, 19–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnusson, D. (Ed.), (1981). Towards a psychology of situations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mainardi-Peron, E., Baroni, M. R., Job, R., & Salmaso, P. (1985). Cognitive factors and communicative strategies in recalling unfamiliar places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 325–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marans, R. W., & Ahrentzen, S. (1987). Developments in research design, data collection, and analysis: Quantitative methods. In E. H. Zube & G. T. Moore (Eds.), Advances in environment, behavior, and design, Vol. 1. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 89–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. J., & O’Kelly, M. (1983). Estimating shopping choice models from travel diary data. Professional Geographer, 35, 440–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moar, I., & Bowers, G. H. (1983). Inconsistency in spatial knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 11, 107–113.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. T. (1979). Knowing about environmental knowing: The current state of theory and research on environment cognition. Environment and Behavior, 11, 33–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. T. (1987). Environment and behavior research in North America: History, developments, and unresolved issues. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nasar, J. L. (1983). Adult viewers7 preferences in residential scenes: A study of the relationship of environmental attributes to preference. Environment and Behavior, 15, 589–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohno, R. (1986). Notion of duality in visual systems and its implications for environmental design. In W. H. Ittelson, M. Asaki, & M. Ker (Eds.), Crosscultural research in environment and behavior. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pailhous, J., Lepecq, & Peruch, P. (1987). Human spatial reference systems. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and men (Vol. 2). Dordretch, The Netherlands: Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Passini, R. (1984). Wax/finding in architecture. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peruch, P., Pailhous, J., & Deutsch, C. (1985). How do we locate ourselves on a map: A method for analyzing self-location processes. Acta Psychologica, 60, 71–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Presson, C. C., & Hazelrigg, M. D. (1984). Building spatial representations through primary and secondary learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 716–722.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Purcell, A. T. (1986). Environmental perception and affect: A schema discrepancy model. Environment and Behavior, 18, 3–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieser, J. J., Lockman, J. J., & Pick, H. L. Jr. (1980). The role of visual experience in knowledge of spatial layout. Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 185–190.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rieser, J. J., Guth, D. A., & Hill, E. W. (1988). Sensitivity to perspective structure while walking without vision. Perception (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Ward, L. M. (1982). Environmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 651–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Ward, L. M., & Pratt, G. (1981). The affective quality attributed to environments. Environment and Behavior, 13, 259–288.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. J. (1980). An information storage model for distance cognition. Environment and Behavior, 12, 183–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, A. W., & White, S. H. (1975). The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 10). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sime, J. D. (1986). Creating places or designing spaces? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6, 49–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. R., Clark, W. A. V., Huff, D., & Shapiro, P. (1979). A decision making and search model for intraurban migration. Geographical Analysis, 11, 1–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. R., Pellegrino, J. W., & Golledge, R. G. (1982). Computational process modeling of spatial cognition and behavior. Geographical Analysis, 14, 305–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, A. H. (1980). Cognition and shopping choice: A multidimensional scaling approach. Environment and Planning A, 12, 1235–1251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, C., Blades, M. (1986). Pattern and process: A review essay on the relationship between behavioural geography and environmental psychology. Progress in Human Geography, 10, 230–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, C., & Darvizeh, Z. (1981). The case for developing a cognitive environmental psychology that does not underestimate the abilities of young children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1, 21–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, C., & Weetman, M. (1981). The microgenesis of cognitive maps: A longitudinal study of new residents of an urban area. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 6, 375–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokols, D. (1978). Environmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 253–295.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 560–589.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W., & Stasz, C. (1980). Individual differences in procedures for knowledge acquisition from maps. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 137–175.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Timmermans, H. (1982). Consumer choice of shopping center: An information integration approach. Regional Studies, 16, 171–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timmermans, H., van der Heijden, R., & Westerveld, H. (1982a). The identification of factors influencing destination choice: An application of the repertory grid methodology. Transportation, 11, 189–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timmermans, H., van der Heijden, R., & Westerveid, H. (1982b). Perception of urban retailing environments: An empirical analysis of consumer information and usage fields. Geoforum, 13, 27–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1983). Categories of environmental scenes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 121–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to the natural environment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and environment (Vol. 6). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, L. M., & Russell, J. A. (1981). The psychological representation of molar physical environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 121–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisman, J. (1981). Evaluating architectural legibility: Wayfinding in the built environment. Environment and Behavior, 13, 189–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, L., &Baum, A. (1987). Physiological aspects of environment-behavior relationships. In E. H. Zube & G. T. Moore (Eds.), Advances in environment, behavior, and design, Vol. 1. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, A. W. (1987). Behavior settings reconsidered: Temporal stages, internal dynamics, and context. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gärling, T., Golledge, R.G. (1989). Environmental Perception and Cognition. In: Zube, E.H., Moore, G.T. (eds) Advance in Environment, Behavior, and Design. Advances in Environment, Behavior, and Design, vol 2. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-0717-4_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-0717-4_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-8047-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-0717-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics