Skip to main content

Tempo and Rhythm

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Springer Handbook of Auditory Research ((SHAR,volume 36))

Abstract

It is a remarkable feat that listeners develop stable representations for auditory events, given the varied, and often ambiguous, temporal patterning of acoustic energy received by the ears. The focus of this chapter is on empirical and theoretical approaches to tempo and rhythm, two aspects of the temporal patterning of sound that are fundamental to musical communication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abel SM (1972) Discrimination of temporal gaps. J Acoust Soc Am 52:519–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alcock KJ, Passingham RE, Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F (2000) Pitch and timing abilities in inherited speech and language impairment. Brain Lang 75:34–46.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Allan LG (1979) The perception of time. Percept Psychophys 26:340–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews MW, Dowling WJ, Bartlett JC, Halpern AR (1998) Identification of speeded and slowed familiar melodies by younger, middle-aged, and older musicians and nonmusicians. Psychol Aging 13:462–471.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberger J (1980) Cognitive structuring in the apprehension and description of simple rhythms. Arch Psychol 48:171–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes R, Jones MR (2000) Expectancy, attention, and time. Cogn Psychol 41:254–311.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin WE (1984) A theory of musical meter. Music Percept 1:355–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergeson T, Trehub S (2002) Absolute pitch and tempo in mothers’ songs to infants. Psychol Sci 13:72–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bharucha J, Pryor JH (1986) Disrupting the isochrony underlying rhythm: an asymmetry in discrimination. Percept Psychophys 40:137–141.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bigand E (1997) Perceiving musical stability: the effect of tonal structure, rhythm, and musical expertise. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 23:808–822.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton TL (1894) Rhythm. Am J Psychol 6:145–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boltz M (1989) Rhythm and “good endings”: effects of temporal structure on tonality judgments. Percept Psychophys 46:9–17.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boltz M (1991) Some structural determinants of melody recall. Mem Cogn 19:239–251.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boltz M (1994) Changes in internal tempo and effects on the learning and remembering of event durations. J Exp Psychol: Learning Mem Cogn 20:1154–1171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boltz M (1998) The processing of temporal and nontemporal information in the remembering of event durations and musical structure. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 24:1087–1104.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boltz M, Jones MR (1986) Does rule recursion make melodies easier to reproduce? If not, what does? Cogn Psychol 18:389–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church RM (2003) A concise introduction to scalar timing theory. In Meck WH (ed), Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke E (1999) Rhythm and timing in music. In Deutsch D (ed), The Psychology of Music, 2nd ed. New York: Academic Press, pp. 473–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collyer CE, Broadbent HA, Church, RM (1994) Preferred rates of repetitive tapping and categorical time production. Percept Psychophys 55:443–453.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper GW, Meyer LB (1960) The Rhythmic Structure of Music. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Creelman CD (1962) Human discrimination of auditory duration. J Acoust Soc Am 34:582–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crocker RL (2000) An Introduction to Gregorian Chant. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalla Bella S, Peretz I, Rousseau L, Gosselin N (2001) A developmental study of the affective value of tempo and mode in music. Cognition 80:1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawe LA, Platt JR, Racine RJ (1993) Harmonic accents in inference of metrical structure and perception of rhythm patterns. Percept Psychophys 54:794–807.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desain P, Honing H (1999) Computational models of beat induction: the rule-based approach. J New Music Res 28:29–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch D (1980) The processing of structured and unstructured tonal sequences. Percept Psychophys 28:381–389.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dowling WJ, Lung KM, Herrbold S (1987) Aiming attention in pitch and time in the perception of interleaved melodies. Percept Psychophys 41:642–656.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Drake C (1993) Perceptual and performed accents in musical sequences. Bull Psychonomic Soc 31:107–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake C, Botte MC (1993) Tempo sensitivity in auditory sequences: evidence for a multiple-look model. Percept Psychophys 54:277–286.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Drake C, Palmer C (1993) Accent structure in music performance. Music Percept 10:343–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake C, Dowling WJ, Palmer C (1991) Accent structures in the reproduction of simple tunes by children and adult pianists. Music Percept 8:315–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake C, Jones MR, Baruch C (2000) The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition 77:251–288.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Duchek, JM, Balota, DA, Ferraro FR (1994) Component analysis of a rhythmic finger tapping task in individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type and in individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 8:218–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duke RA (1989) Musicians’ perception of beat in monotonic stimuli. J Res Music Ed 37:61–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eck D (2002) Finding downbeats with a relaxation oscillator. Psychol Res 66:18–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis RJ, Jones MR (2009) The role of accent salience and joint accent structure in meter perception. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 35:264–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Essens PJ, Povel DJ (1985) Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Percept Psychophys 37:1–7.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Farnsworth P, Block H, Waterman W (1934) Absolute tempo. J Gen Psychol 10:230–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse P (1956) Les structures rythmiques. Louvain, Belgium: Publications Universitaires de Louvain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse P (1963) The Psychology of Time. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse P (1982) Rhythm and tempo. In Deutsch D (ed), The Psychology of Music. Orlando, FL: Academic, pp. 149–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friberg A, Sundberg, J (1995) Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous sequence. J Acoust Soc Am 98:2524–2531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friberg A, Sundström (2002) Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance: evidence for a common rhythmic pattern. Music Percept 19:333–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frischeisen-Köhler, I (1933) The personal tempo and its inheritance. Character Pers 1:301–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon L, Peretz I (2003) Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgements in equitone melodies Cogn Emotion 17:25–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner WR (1974) The Processing of Information and Structure. Oxford: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getty DJ (1975) Discrimination of short temporal intervals: a comparison of two models. Percept Psychophys 18:1– 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geuze RH, Kalverboer, AF (1994) Tapping a rhythm: a problem of timing for children who are clumsy and dyslexic? Adapt Phys Act Q 11:203–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon J (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychol Rev 84:279–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon J, Church RM, Meck WH (1984) Scalar timing in memory. Ann NY Acad Sci 423:52–77.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grahn JA, Brett M (2009) Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 45:54–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grahn JA, McAuley JD (2009) Neural bases of individual differences in beat perception. NeuroImage 47:1894–1903.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene LS, Williams, HG (1993) Age-related differences in timing control of repetitive movement: application of the Wing-Kristofferson model. Res Q Exerc Sport 64:32–38.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin S (2001a) Discriminating time intervals presented in sequences marked by visual signals. Percept Psychophys 63:1214–1228.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin S (2001b) From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychol Bull 47:22–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grondin S, McAuley JD (2009) Duration discrimination in crossmodal sequences. Perception 38:1542–1559.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern AR (1988) Perceived and imagined tempos of familiar songs. Music Percept 6:193–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handel S (1984) Using polyrhythms to study rhythm. Music Percept 1:465–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handel S (1989) Listening: An Introduction to the Perception of Auditory Events. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handel S (1998) The interplay between metric and figural rhythmic organization. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 24:1546–1561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handel S, Lawson GR (1983) The contextual nature of rhythmic interpretation. Percept Psychophys 34:103–120.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Handel S, Oshinsky, JS (1981) The meter of syncopated auditory polyrhythms. Percept Psychophys 30:1–9.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hannon EE, Johnson SP (2005) Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: implications for musical structure learning. Cogn Psychol 50:354–377.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hannon EE, Trehub SE (2005) Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychol Sci 16:48–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hannon EE, Snyder JS, Eerola T, Krumhansl CL (2004) The role of melodic and temporal cues in perceiving musical meter. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 30:956–974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrel TW (1937) Factors influencing preference and memory for auditory rhythm. J Gen Psychol 17:63–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hébert S, Cuddy LL (2002) Detection of metric structure in auditory figural patterns. Percept Psychophys 64:909–918.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh IJ, Monahan CB, Grant KW, Singh PG (1990) Studies in auditory timing: 1. Simple patterns. Percept Psychophys 47:215–226.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huron D, Royal M (1996) What is melodic accent? Converging evidence from musical practice. Music Percept 13:489–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iversen JR, Patel AD (2008) The beat alignment test (BAT): surveying beat processing abilities in the general population. In Adachi et al. (eds), Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, August 25–29. Adelaide: Causal Productions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iversen JR, Patel AD, Ohgushi K (2008) Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. J Acoust Soc Am 124:2263–2271.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry RB, Hazeltine RE (1995) Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 21:3–18.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry RB, Keele SW (1989) Timing functions of the cerebellum. J Cogn Neurosci 6:136–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890) The Principles of Psychology, Vol. I. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR (1976) Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychol Rev 83:323–355.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR (1981) A tutorial on some issues and methods in serial pattern research. Percept Psychophys 30:492–504.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR (1987) Dynamic pattern structure in music: recent theory and research. Percept Psychophys 41:621–634.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR (1993) Dynamics of musical patterns: how do melody and rhythm fit together? In Tighe TJ, Dowling WJ (eds), Psychology and Music: The Understanding of Melody and Rhythm. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 67–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Boltz M (1989) Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychol Rev 96:459–491.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, McAuley JD (2005) Time judgments in global temporal contexts. Percept Psychophys 67:398–417.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Pfordresher PQ (1997) Tracking melodic events using joint accent structure. Can J Exp Psychol 51:271–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Ralston JT (1991) Some influences of accent structure on melody recognition. Mem Cogn 19:8–20.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Yee W (1997) Sensitivity to time change: the role of context and skill. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 23:693–709.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Kidd GR, Wetzel R (1981) Evidence for rhythmic attention. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 7:1059–1073.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Boltz M, Kidd GR (1982) Controlled attending as a function of melodic and temporal context. Percept Psychophys 32:211–218.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Boltz M, Klein JM (1993) Expected endings and judged duration. Mem Cogn 21:646–665.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones MR, Moynihan H, Mackenzie N, Puente J (2002) Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychol Sci 13:313–319.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keele SW, Nicoletti R, Ivry RI, Pokorny RA (1989) Mechanisms of perceptual timing: beat-based or interval-based judgments? Psychol Res 50:221–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein JM, Jones MR (1996) Effects of attentional set and rhythmic complexity on attending. Percept Psychophys 58:34–46.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krampe R, Mayr U, Kliegl R (2005) Timing, sequencing, and executive control in repetitive movement production. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 31:379–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Large EW, Jones MR (1999) The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol Rev 107:119–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Large EW, Kolen JF 1994. Resonance and the perception of musical meter. Connection Sci 6:177–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Large EW, Palmer C (2002) Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cogn Sci 26:1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Large EW, Fink P, Kelso JAS (2002) Tracking simple and complex sequences. Psychol Res 66:3–17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerdahl F, Jackendoff R (1983) A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levitin DJ, Cook PR (1996) Memory for musical tempo: additional evidence that auditory memory is absolute. Percept Psychophys 58:927–935.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • London J (2002) Cognitive constraints on metric systems: some observations and hypotheses. Music Percept 19:529–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • London J (2004) Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longuet-Higgens C, Lee C (1982) The perception of musical rhythms. Perception 11:115–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattel MS, Meck WH (2000) Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. BioEssays 22:94–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAuley JD (1995) Perception of time as phase: toward an adaptive-oscillator model of rhythmic pattern processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAuley JD, Jones MR (2003) Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: a comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 29:1102–1125.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAuley JD, Kidd GR (1998) Effect of deviations from temporal expectations on tempo discrimination of isochronous tone sequences. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 24:1786–1800.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McAuley JD, Semple P (1999) The effect of tempo and musical experience on perceived beat. Aust J Psychol 51:176–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAuley JD, Jones MR, Holub S, Johnston HM, Miller NS (2006) The time of our lives: lifespan development of timing and event tracking. J Exp Psychol: Gen 135:348–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meck WH (1996) Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Cogn Brain Res 3:227–242.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meck WH (2003) Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meck WH (2005) Neuropsychology of timing and time perception. Brain Cogn 58:1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michon JA (1964) Studies on subjective duration: I. Differential sensitivity in the perception of repeated temporal intervals. Acta Psychol 22:441–450.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller NS, McAuley JD (2005) Tempo sensitivity in isochronous tone sequences: the multiple-look model revisited. Percept Psychophys 67:1150–1160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mishima J (1956) On the factors of mental tempo. Japanese Psychol Res 4:27–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monahan CB, Kendall RA, Carterette EC (1987) The effect of melodic and temporal contour on recognition memory for pitch change. Percept Psychophys 41:576–600.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore-Ede MC, Sulzman FM, Fuller CA (1982) The Clocks that Time Us: Physiology of the Circadian Timing System. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narmour E (1996) Analyzing form and measuring perceptual content in Mozart’s Sonata K. 282: a new theory of parametric analogues. Music Percept 13:728–741.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogden RT, Collier GL (1999) On detecting and modeling deterministic drift in long run sequences of tapping data. Comm Stat Theory Methods 28:977–987.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oshinsky JS, Handel S (1978) Syncopated auditory polyrhythms: discontinuous reversals in meter interpretation. J Acoust Soc Am 63:936–939.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer C, Krumhansl CL (1990) Mental representations of musical meter. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 16:728–741.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Parncutt R (1994) A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Percept 11:409–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler H (2001) Perception and production of brief durations: beat-based versus interval-based timing. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 27:485–493.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Patel AD, Iversen JR, Chen Y, Repp BH (2005) The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat. Exp Brain Res 163:226–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfordresher, PQ (2003) The role of melodic and rhythmic accents in musical structure. Music Percept 20:431–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pöppel E (1997) A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends Cogn Sci 1:56–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Povel DJ, Essens PJ (1985) Perception of temporal patterns. Music Percept 2:411–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povel DJ, Okkerman H (1981) Accents in equitone sequences. Percept Psychophys 30:565–572.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Repp BH (2003) Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: the synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. J Mot Behav 35:355–370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp BH (2005) Rate limits of on-beat and off-beat tapping with simple auditory rhythms: 2. The roles of different kinds of accent. Music Percept 23:165–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Repp BH, Windsor WL, Desain P (2002) Effects of tempo on the timing of simple musical rhythms. Music Percept 19:565–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rimoldi HJA (1951) Personal tempo. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 46:280–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross J, Houtsma AJM (1994) Discrimination of auditory temporal patterns. Percept Psychophys 56:19–26.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau L, Rousseau R (1996) Stop-reaction time and the internal clock. Percept Psychophys 58:434–448.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schulze HH (1978) The detectability of local and global displacements in regular rhythmic patterns. Psychol Res 40:173–181.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schulze HH (1989) The perception of temporal deviations in isochronic patterns. Percept Psychophys 45:291–296.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer L (1981) Performances of Chopin, Bach and Bartok: studies in motor programming. Cogn Psychol 13:327–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloboda JA (1983) The communication of musical metre in piano performance. Q J Exp Psychol A 35:377–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Small AM, Campbell RA (1962) Temporal differential sensitivity for auditory stimuli. Am J Psychol 75:401–410.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith KC, Cuddy LL (1989) Effects of metric and harmonic rhythm on the detection of pitch alterations in melodic sequences. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 15:457–471.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith KC, Cuddy LL, Upitis R (1994) Figural and metric understanding of rhythm. Psychol Music 22:117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smoll FL, Schutz RW (1978) Relationships among measures of preferred tempo and motor rhythm. Percept Mot Skills 46:883–94.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder JS, Krumhansl CL (2001) Tapping to ragtime: cues to pulse finding. Music Percept 18:455–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder JS, Hannon EE, Large EW, Christiansen MH (2006) Synchronization and continuation tapping to complex meters. Music Percept 24:135–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steedman MJ (1977) The perception of musical rhythm and metre. Perception 6:555–569.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stern W (1900) Das psychisch Tempo. In Uber psychologie der individuellen differenzen. Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens LT (1886) On the time sense. Mind 11:393–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szelag E, von Steinbüchel N, Reiser M, Gilles de Langen E, Pöppel E (1996) Temporal constraints in processing of nonverbal rhythmic patterns. Acta Neurobiol Exp 56:215–225.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Temperley D (2001) The Cognition of Basic Musical Structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Temperley D, Bartlette C (2002) Parallelism as a factor in metrical analysis. Music Percept 20:117–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • ten Hoopen G, Akerboom S (1983) The subjective tempo difference between interaural and monaural sequences as a function of sequence length. Percept Psychophys 34:465–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ten Hoopen G, Boelaarts L, Gruisen A, Apon I, Donders K, Mul N, Akerboom S (1994) The detection of anisochrony in monaural and interaural sound sequences. Percept Psychophys 56:110–120.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomassen JM (1982) Melodic accent: experiments and a tentative model J Acoust. Soc Am 71:1596–1605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson JM, Goswami U (2008) Rhythmic processing in children with developmental dyslexia: auditory and motor rhythms link to reading and spelling. J Physiol Paris 102:120–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toiviainen P (1998) An interactive MIDI accompanist. Comput Music J 22:63–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toiviainen P, Snyder JS (2003) Tapping to Bach: resonance-based modeling of pulse. Music Percept 21:43–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Noorden L, Moelants D (1999) Resonance in the perception of musical pulse. J Music Res 28:43–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanneste S, Pouthas V, Wearden J (2001) Temporal control of rhythmic performance: a comparison between young and old adults. Exp Aging Res 27:83–102.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vierordt K (1868) Der Zeitsinn nuch Versuchen. Tubingen: H. Laupp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vos PG, van Assen M, Franek M (1997) Perceived tempo change is dependent on base tempo and direction of change: evidence for a generalized version of Schulze’s (1978) internal beat model. Psychol Res 59:240–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallin J (1911) Experimental studies of rhythm and time. Psychol Rev 18:100–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams HG, Woollacott MH, Ivry RB (1992) Timing and motor control in clumsy children. J Mot Behav 24:165–172.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wing A (1980) The long and short of timing in response sequences. In Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds), Tutorials in Motor Behavior. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing AM, Kristofferson AB (1973) The timing of inter-response intervals. Percept Psychophys 13:455–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodrow H (1909) A quantitative study of rhythm. Arch Psychol 14:1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodrow H (1911) The role of pitch in rhythm. Psychol Rev 18:54–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodrow H (1951) Time perception. In Stevens SS (ed), Handbook of Experimental Psychology. New York: John Wiley, pp. 1224–1236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt W (1874) Grudzüge der physiologischen psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Many thanks to Mari Riess Jones, Laura Dilley, Molly Henry, and Nathan Miller for detailed comments on an earlier version of this chapter. Thanks also to Laura Dilley for help with figures. The author received support for this work from the National Science Foundation (BCS 0818271).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Devin McAuley .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

McAuley, J.D. (2010). Tempo and Rhythm. In: Riess Jones, M., Fay, R., Popper, A. (eds) Music Perception. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, vol 36. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6114-3_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics