Skip to main content

Historical Perspectives in the Study of Action Control

  • Chapter
Action Control

Part of the book series: SSSP Springer Series in Social Psychology ((SSSOC))

Abstract

In his summary of a socio-genetic theory of voluntary regulation, Leontiev (1932) describes a strategy the Chinese mailman used to avoid getting distracted while delivering an urgent telegram. “He organizes his own behavior, creating for himself additional stimuli. He hangs a number of subjects — a piece of coal, a pen, and some pepper on the end of a short rod. This he keeps before his eyes on the road. This will remind him that he must fly like a bird, run as if he was stepping on hot coals or had burnt himself with pepper” (Leontiev, 1932, p. 57). This example illustrates a strategy of voluntary regulation which presumably facilitates cognition-behavior consistency. Whether or not a cognition suggesting a certain action results in its enactment depends on the actor’s ability to apply appropriate self-regulatory strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ach, N. (1905). Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung mit einem Anhange. Über das Hippsche Chronoskop. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament. Leipzig: Quelle und Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens. In E. Abderhalden (Ed.), Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden. Bd. VI. Berlin: Urban & Schwarzenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1973). Attitudinal and normative variables as predictors of specific behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J.W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, N.J.: Van Nostrand, 1958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckmann, J., & Kuhl, J. (1984). Altering information to gain action control: Functional aspects of human information-processing in decision-making. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 223–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Düker, H. (1925). Über das Gesetz der speziellen Determination. Untersuchungen zur Psychologie, Philosophie und Pädagogik, 5, 97–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1902). Grundzüge der Psychologie. I. Leipzig: Veit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortlage, K. (1855). System der Psychologie. Leipzig: F. A. Brockhaus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerdessen, H. (1932). Die Entwicklung der Willensbetätigung auf die Eigenschaften der Bezugsvorstellungen. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie. (Suppl. 2). 1-102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harriman, P.L. (1947). The new dictionary of psychology. New York: Philosophical Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion. 1, 283–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904). The principles of psychology. Vol. II. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H.H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesler, C. A. (1971). The psychology of commitment. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Ajzen, J. (1983). Bias and error in human judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983). Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1984). Volitional aspects of achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action-control. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in Experimental Personality Research (Vol. 13). (pp. 99–171). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1983). Handlungskontrolle und Umfang der Informationsverarbeitung: Wahl einer vereinfachten (nicht-optimalen) Entscheidungsregel zugunsten rascher Handlungsbereitschaft. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 14, 241–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kukla, A. (1972). Foundations of an attributional theory of performance. Psychological Review, 79, 454–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Külpe, O. (1893). Grundriß der Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontiev, A.N. (1932). The development of voluntary attention in the child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 40, 52–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1922). Das Problem der Willensmessung und das Grundgesetz der Assoziation. I und II. Psychologische Forschung, 1, 191–302 and 2, 65-140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1926). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs-und Affekt-Psychologie. II.: Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 330–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in the social sciences. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindworsky, J. (1929). The training of the will. Milwaukee: Bruce. (Original work published in 1923).

    Google Scholar 

  • Messer, A. (1934). Psychologie. Leipzig: F. Meier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, W.-U. (1973). Leistungsmotiv und Ursachenerklärung von Erfolg und Mißerfolg. Stuttgart: Klett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. (1911). Note complémentaire. Archive de Psychologie, 10, 113–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A., & Prüm, E. (1910), Etude expérimentale sur le choix volontaire et ses antécédents immédiats. Travaux du Laboratoire de Psychologie expérimentale de l’Université de Louvain, 1 (2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K.-H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, E. (1932). Beiträge zur Lehre von der Determination. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 84, 43–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Münsterberg, H. (1888). Die Willenshandlung. Freiburg, FRG: Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. W., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1980). Twelve issues for cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 4, 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohracher, H. (1932). Theorie des Willens. Zeitschrift für Psychologie (Suppl. 21).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology. 7, 605–753.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simoneit, M. (1926). Willenshemmung und Assoziation. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 100, 161–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1980). Human motivation. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion process in achievement-related contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1211–1220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundtm, W. (1896). Grundriß der Psychologic Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziehen, Th. (1920). Leitfaden der physiologischen Psychologie. Jena: Gustav Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J. (1985). Historical Perspectives in the Study of Action Control. In: Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J. (eds) Action Control. SSSP Springer Series in Social Psychology. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-69748-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-69746-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics