Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research 7/2022

Open Access 24-02-2022

‘Like holding the axe on who should live or not’: adolescents’ and adults’ perceptions of valuing children’s health states using a standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L

Auteurs: Mimmi Åström, Helen Conte, Jenny Berg, Kristina Burström

Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research | Uitgave 7/2022

Abstract

Purpose

There is an increasing interest for using qualitative methods to investigate peoples’ cognitive process when asked to value health states. A standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L instrument was recently developed. Little is known regarding how people think, reason, and feel when asked to value health states for children. The aim was to explore how adolescents and adults perceive the task of valuing children’s health states using the standardised valuation protocol.

Methods

This was a qualitative study where adults (n = 10) and adolescents (n = 10) from the general population participated in individual video-interviews. Initially, participants reported their own health with the EQ-5D-3L instrument. Then they were asked to complete several valuations tasks for a 10-year-old child according to the standardised valuation protocol, followed by a semi-structured interview with open-ended questions to further explore participants’ perceptions. A qualitative content analysis was performed.

Results

The two main categories that emerged from the data were ‘Thoughts and feelings when valuing children’s health states’ and ‘Strategies when valuing children’s health states’. Participants expressed feeling doubt, awfulness and being reluctant to trade-off life years, and questioned who has the right to value health states for children. Experience and point of view were strategies participants used to complete the valuation tasks.

Conclusion

The findings from the present study can contribute to the understanding and interpretation of quantitative results where the standardised valuation protocol has been used to derive values for the EQ-5D-Y-3L. Furthermore, results of the study support the feasibility of including adolescents in valuation studies.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s11136-022-03107-0.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

There are several instruments for measuring health-related quality of life (HRQoL) among children and adolescents [1]. One of these instruments is the EQ-5D-Y-3L, a generic instrument developed to measure self-reported health among children from the age of eight [2, 3]. The first part of the instrument, the descriptive system, covers five dimensions of health (mobility; looking after myself; doing usual activities; having pain or discomfort; feeling worried, sad or unhappy) with three severity levels. By combining the dimensions and the severity levels, 243 unique health states can be formed. For example, health state 33,333 represents ‘a lot of problems’ in all dimensions. The second part is a visual analogue scale (EQ VAS), where the respondent rates his/her overall health between 0 (worst imaginable health) and 100 (best imaginable health) [2].
HRQoL instruments can be used to combine length of life and HRQoL into quality-adjusted life years (QALYs). Each of the health states is converted into a single value, using valuation methods such as the time trade-off (TTO), the standard gamble (SG) and the visual analogue scale (VAS) [4]. Eliciting values from children themselves or from a proxy have yielded different results, where the magnitude depends on for example valuation method and the health states valued [5]. To guide the elicitation of values for the EQ-5D-Y-3L instrument, a standardised protocol valuation protocol has been published, which includes the valuation methods TTO and discrete choice experiment (DCE) [6]. In the TTO method, respondents are asked to choose, for a 10-year-old child, between being in a specified health state for X number of years and a shorter time in full health. Using DCE, respondents are asked to choose between two health states which they prefer for a 10-year-old child. Two value sets have been published using this standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L, from Slovenia [7] and Japan [8]. In both value sets, adults valued pain or discomfort as the most important health dimension for a child.
There are several methodological and practical challenges concerning valuation of health states for adults, and these are even more pronounced when it comes to children’s health states. Central considerations are whether to derive health state values by asking respondents to value their own health state (experience-based) or to value described health states (hypothetical) and choosing which valuation methods to use [9, 10]. Previous methodological studies have shown that using the TTO and DCE methods have resulted in higher mean values, i.e., same health states being less severe, for children compared to adults [11]. Lipman et al. [12] point on a methodological issue also when valuing hypothetical health states: the individual perspective, as in the protocol for the adult versions of the EQ-5D, where respondents should think of ‘someone like you’ being in the hypothetical health states, and the child perspective, as in the protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L, i.e., to value health for ‘someone else’, a 10-year-old [12].
The need to investigate the cognitive process people go through when valuing health states was recognised in the first valuation study of the adult version EQ-5D-3L [13], and the burden on respondents and the complexity of the tasks have been emphasised [14, 15]. Considering alternative approaches, and hence future updates of the standardised valuation protocol might be of importance, e.g., regarding the relevance of using values from adults or younger age groups for the valuation of children’s health states [8]. Using qualitative methods when planning and organizing valuation exercises is a key element [16]. Results from qualitative studies might therefore contribute with insights that could support further development of the standardised protocol. There are few qualitative studies investigating peoples’ thoughts and feelings when valuing health states for children, especially in relation to the standardised valuation protocol [17] and few studies addressing the difference between child and adult perspective [12]. Hence, the aim of the present study was to explore how adolescents and adults perceive the task of valuing children’s health states using the standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L.

Materials and methods

Study design

A qualitative study was carried out where adults and adolescents from the Swedish general population participated in individual interviews to explore how they perceived the task of valuing health states according to the standard valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L instrument. A qualitative content analysis was performed [18]. The Consolidated criteria for Reporting Qualitative research (COREQ) checklist was applied [19] (Online Resource Appendix 1).

Setting and study participants

Recruitment of participants was based on convenience sampling [20] and participants were enrolled on a first come, first served basis. Adults were recruited by advertising the study on Karolinska Institutet’s webpage, and adolescents by sending an e-mail to all students aged 15 years or older in a school in Stockholm. Adults and adolescents who replied received a letter of information and a form for informed consent. Participants had to speak and understand Swedish, which was assessed by the interviewer in the beginning of each interview.
For the main data collection, 20 individual interviews were conducted: ten adults (90% females; median age 39.5; median EQ VAS score 82.5) and ten adolescents (70% females; median age 17; median EQ VAS score 77.5). Background characteristics of study participants, parental status, participants’ experience with children, and how they reported their own health with the EQ-5D-3L and EQ VAS are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Background characteristics study participants
Participant
Sex
Age
EQ-5D-3L health state
EQ VAS score
Experience of children*
Being a parent
P1
Female
17
11222
60
Yes
 
P2
Female
17
11111
85
Yes
 
P3
Male
17
11111
85
Yes
 
P4
Female
16
11111
70
Yes
 
P5
Female
17
11112
95
Yes
 
P6
Male
18
11111
80
Yes
 
P7
Male
17
11112
75
Yes
 
P8
Female
16
11122
75
Yes
 
P9
Female
16
11112
80
Yes
 
P10
Female
16
11112
75
Yes
 
P11
Female
42
11111
90
 
No
P12
Female
43
11111
80
 
Yes
P13
Female
24
11111
90
 
No
P14
Female
41
11111
70
 
Yes
P15
Male
47
11111
90
 
Yes
P16
Female
22
11222
60
 
No
P17
Female
33
11112
50
 
Yes
P18
Female
26
11122
85
 
No
P19
Female
40
11111
90
 
Yes
P20
Female
39
11111
80
 
Yes
*For example baby sitting or organising activities for children

Data collection

Pilot interviews were conducted (Online Resource Appendix 2). The main data collection took place between February and April 2021. The first author, who had previous experience with interviewing children [21] and adults, conducted the interviews which lasted between 44 and 79 min.
The interview began with the participant reporting their own health with the EQ-5D-3L. This was followed by the completion of the valuation tasks according to the standardised valuation protocol [6]. The interviewer and the participants had their video cameras on during the interview through Zoom to facilitate communication and the interviewer’s screen was shared to visualise the tasks. Participants were asked to confirm all choices made in the valuation tasks. The latter part of the interview was semi-structured, with identical open-ended questions to both groups (Online Resource Appendix 3). Ethical approval was granted by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (Dnr: 2020-03753, 2020-05390).

Data analysis

All interviews were recorded and transcribed verbatim. An inductive qualitative content analysis in NVivo 1.4.1 was performed, where categories were derived from the raw data (from codes to sub-, generic- and main categories), guided by three phases: preparation, organising and reporting [18]. The process of moving from sub-categories to generic categories and finally to main categories is illustrated in Fig. 1 (details in Online Resource Appendix 4).

Results

Two main categories emerged from the data: ‘Thoughts and feelings when valuing children’s health states’ and ‘Strategies when valuing children’s health states’ (Fig. 1). These categories were intertwined, as how adolescents and adults thought and felt influenced what strategies they used and the other way around. See Online Resource Appendix 5 for quotes in addition to those below.

Main category 1: thoughts and feelings when valuing children’s health states

This main category contained three generic categories. The level of emotional involvement varied and a variety of thoughts and feeling arose among the participants depending on the severity of the health state valued and the valuation method.

Understanding the trade-off between life years and health

In general, participants understood quickly that their task was to trade between quantity and quality of life in the TTO task.
‘Yes, it becomes more black and white. Compared to when you thought for yourself, then you reasoned… you avoid having this pain… But here, it just becomes these years we take away from the child’s life’ R12, adult
Many of the participants sought the interviewer’s confirmation regarding the fact that both Life A and Life B are followed by death in this task, and it was clear that most participants thought this was offensive. Valuing health for a 10-year-old child was brought up by most participants as something that made the tasks difficult.
‘Except from feeling a bit horrible at times. It felt like, God, what a horrible thought, but if I have to choose, I choose like this. So, I think it was… it was interesting. It was fun to… get an eye-opener’
R13, adult
‘Oh. You feel so grotesque if you think about [shortening] a 10-year-old’s life’ R10, adolescent
Although, most of the participants expressed these feelings, a majority traded life years for the more severe health states. A few participants argued that avoiding suffering for the child was most important, no matter how short the life for that child became. It was also clear that the initial reaction, i.e., unwillingness to trade-off life years, did not necessarily result in non-trader behaviour.
‘…when it concerns a child, it is almost like you get a bad conscience to cast away life, and this might not have been the case if it had been an adult. So, for example, if it had been for your partner it might also have been easier compared to a child, actually’ R20, adult
One specific trigger observed among most of the participants was the confirmation box in the TTO task. When they reached the point of indifference between Life A and Life B, the confirmation box appears with the following text ‘Your response suggests that to avoid a 10-year-old child being in this health state for 10 years you are willing to give up X year(s)’.
After understanding the idea behind the tasks, many reacted to the valuation tasks and especially the TTO task. Most participants expressed being uncomfortable when making the decision, especially shortening the life for the child. It was also clear that participants felt a responsibility when completing the tasks. Participants also expressed feeling horrible and grotesque when completing the TTO task. One participant reacted to the tasks by being worried over how the valuations were going to be used in practice.
‘No, but I thought like this, if you are going to use this for something, physicians or others who will receive this answer and then just: “yes but when to end someone’s life”, but I thought if that will be based on this, because I hope not’ R14, adult
Most of the participants thought that the DCE task was easier to complete compared to the TTO task, foremost because they did not have to shorten the life for a child in the DCE task. Many also recognised the DCE to be more theoretical and more straightforward.

Questioning who should value health

Many of the participants reflected over who has the right to value health for a 10-year-old child, and most found it hard and expressed doubt. Even though participants expressed these feelings and thoughts, all managed to complete the tasks.
‘Why should I… decide which of these… what right do I have to decide, which of these are better or worse, that is what is happening in my brain. Hmm, why should anyone except that child or that parent have the right to say what is better for them?’ R1, adolescent
Not having experience of the health state to be valued, implying not being the right person to value it, was expressed by many. Participants brought up that it is impossible to imagine how a health state is for another person and therefore hesitated to assign a value. Many expressed concerns about completing the valuation in an accurate way. A common reflection was that they would have preferred to value their own health state over valuing health states for someone else.
‘…I cannot identify with these health conditions, I do know people who have had these conditions, but I myself cannot imagine how that would be. So, it is really difficult for me to value these things when I have not experienced it myself’ R1, adolescent

Prioritising mental health and incorporating surroundings

To value health states based on the dimensions and the severity levels in the EQ-5D-Y-3L instrument was in general accepted by the participants.
‘I thought it was good. At least I got a picture of what I thought it [the health state] would sort of imply’ R17, adult
Many reflected over the division of health in physical and mental health. Most participants described mental health aspects and, hence, the dimension feeling worried, sad, or unhappy as most important. Some participants even thought that mental health outweighs all physical health problems.
‘I consider the psychological aspect more, because I feel that psychological problems are much heavier, as that is the root of life’ R4, adolescent
In contrast, one participant reflected over the importance for the individual not to lose their integrity, and therefore thought that the dimension looking after myself was very important. A variation in awareness of one’s views among the participants was observed: some knew that they thought mental aspects were most important; others were surprised about their prioritisation.
Pain was also pointed out as an important dimension. Many expressed that they would not like to see anyone having pain, and some stressed that even more strongly when valuing health for a 10-year-old child. Being unhappy in combination with pain was considered as the worst by several.
‘Yes, basically I remove the first three [dimensions]. The relationship pain and psychological pain, the rest I find is not so important in this context’ R14, adult
Some recognised some health states as implausible. Furthermore, many of the participants struggled and found it unrealistic that the health states were constant in the TTO task as there was no chance for the child to improve. Many participants mentioned social aspects as an important part of life and that they would have liked such a dimension in the EQ-5D-Y-3L.
‘It [a social dimension] would have been something one would want to include, as the social part is quite important’ R20, adult
Participants reflected over the context the imagined 10-year-old child lived in and how that affected the relative importance of health dimensions. The child would have different challenges when coping with different health states depending on the environment.
Participants thought it to be important whether the child had social support, family, and a good situation in school. Some argued that some of the health states would be even worse for adults compared to a child, as parents/guardians might make it possible to cope with some of the health states if they occur when you are 10-year-old.
‘I think it is more difficult for adults to need this support than for children, as children often have their parents present and hmm, and I think it is more difficult for adults to get that kind of help and support’ R1, adolescent

Main category 2: strategies when valuing children’s health states

This main category covered two generic categories. The strategies used by the participants to complete the valuation tasks varied between and within participants. Depending on the health state to be valued or the valuation task, TTO or DCE, some participants employed different strategies while others stuck to the same strategy.

Using experience

Participants used own experience and vicarious experience as strategies to seek understanding and knowledge about the health state. They reflected over their own current health, for example if they had a disease with similar features.
‘Since I myself have been suffering from mental illness during a long period, I would have preferred this short [option]. I would have preferred a shorter life and avoiding… Because even so, you do not want to do anything if you feel so bad. Then I prefer to remove that and to have a better shorter time instead of a longer period that is just hard’
R10, adolescent
A variation was observed among participants: some reflected on their own time as a 10-year-old child; others reflected more generally on different health states they had experienced regardless of age. Participants were also seeking understanding by reflecting on health states people in their surroundings experienced, referred to as vicarious experience. The person they referred to was for example a family member, a relative, someone they had heard of, or even a movie star.
‘Because I saw that movie about Stephen Hawking on Netflix, and it is beautiful, and he had many physical problems, but he did have a really good life’ R14, adult

Adopting a point of view

Participants reflected on whose point of view to adopt when completing the valuation tasks, and how adopting different point of views might yield different values. There was no clear pattern regarding how the actual valuation of the health state was affected by which point of view was adopted, as it was not clear which point of view had been taken for valuing given health states.
The parents’ point of view was mentioned by most of the participants: some argued that from an egoistic point of view, as a parent (regardless of being a parent or not), you would like your child to live as many years as possible; others emphasized the importance of avoiding suffering for your child. Participants reflected both on how the health state for the child would affect the parent and on how a parent would have valued the health state for their own child. Different points of view were used as strategies when valuing. Awareness of how the participants’ decisions were affected by emotional ties was shown by some, especially when imagining their own child.
‘…if it was my own children, then I would like them to live, to maximise time, one is selfish with it. But if you think from the person’s own perspective, then I would say no – why should you live and just suffer?’ R17, adult
‘I tried to think about how I would do if I thought logically. But if it was my own child, logic alone would not matter’ R2, adolescent
Many participants reflected over the different viewpoints that could be adopted but concluded that they could only use their own viewpoint in completing the valuation tasks. Some also recognised that they would have preferred to ask the child him- or herself about their preferences.

Discussion

This interview study provides insights into how adults and adolescents from a general population perceived the task of valuing health states for a 10-year-old child according to the standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L instrument [6]. Participant’s thoughts and feelings influenced the strategies they used, and vice versa. The level of emotional investment was continually changing among some participants depending on the severity of the health state to be valued or which valuation method being used, while others remained in the same emotional state throughout the valuation tasks.
It was clear that participants found the TTO task most offensive and were uncomfortable with completing it, as it incorporates a trade-off between life years and health for a child. Participants would have preferred to value health for themselves or another adult instead. The importance of avoiding suffering for a child was one of the motivations raised by several participants on why they nevertheless ended up trading-off life years.
Participants’ ability to quickly grasp the purpose of trading-off between life years and health is in line with a previous study [22]; in our study, no difference between adults and adolescents was observed regarding understanding the tasks. Hence, our results support the engagement of adolescents from 16 years in completing TTO tasks. Previous studies have shown that adults’ and adolescents’ preferences differ [23, 24] and, therefore, our results are important as they demonstrate the feasibility of capturing adolescents’ values, even though we did not investigate possible differences in values for the health states in our study. When asked to share their thoughts and feelings regarding the valuation tasks, many adolescents and adults expressed feelings of reluctance and doubt when completing the TTO tasks, as reported earlier [15]. Future studies could address potential differences between males and females which was not possible with our convenience sampling.
Valuing health states by the EQ-5D-Y-3L instrument was generally accepted by participants. However, many participants expressed that the EQ-5D-Y-3L did not cover all dimensions important to health, which has been recognised earlier [25].
In the present study, it was clear that mental health was the most important dimension when valuing health. This is in line with previous general population studies where adults and adolescents valued their own health [2628]. Mental and cognitive health were also more important than physical health in a recent study using the TTO [21]. In our study, some participants even pointed out the mental health aspects as paramount, hence, focused only on this dimension. Exclusively making the valuation decision on solely one health dimension has been observed earlier [29]. Participants identifying implausible health states, as in our study, was also found in the study by Karimi et al. [15]. The context the child was growing up in and the social support system, for example support by parents or guardians, were central in our study and has previously been identified as important [15]. Participants reflected over that some health states might even be easier for children than adults to cope with, as children commonly have support by an adult for example with problems in the dimension looking after myself. Social aspects are incorporated in the dimension doing usual activities, but participants wanted social aspects to be a specific dimension.
The question regarding who should value health states is a burning topic, discussed in the literature and recognised as a normative issue [1, 30, 31]. In the standardised valuation protocol [6], the choice of having an adult general population valuing described (hypothetical) health states was mainly motivated by the ‘taxpayer’ argument. The main difference between how values for hypothetical health states are derived for the adult version of the EQ-5D-5L [32] and the EQ-5D-Y-3L [6] is the shift of perspective, from individual to child perspective, as pointed out by Lipman et al. [12]. This change of perspective might, in itself, lead to differences in derived values.
The strategy of using experience to concretise the health state to be valued is recognised from previous studies [15, 22] as is focusing on the future when using the TTO [33, 34]. In the present analysis, the categorization of different types of experience into own and vicarious experience was influenced by Cubi-Molla et al. [35]. Even though it was clearly stated in our study that they were asked to value health states for a 10-year-old child, adopting different point of views was a strategy many participants used.
The location for the interview might affect data, and it is important that the participants feel comfortable [36]. That many participated from home could potentially have enabled participants to express themselves more. Advantages, in terms of increased participation and flexibility, and disadvantages, in terms of loss of visual cues and difficulty in building a positive atmosphere, have been recognised using digital aids such as Zoom for interviewing [37, 38]. Weak internet connection was sometimes a challenge in our study. To validate data in the present study the interviewer asked for confirmation that she had understood the participants in a correct manner [38]. A limitation of our study was using the strategy of first come first served when enrolling participants, this could possibly have influenced the results as it resulted in the inclusion of few (n = 4) males. However, to the best of our knowledge, there are no previous literature showing differences between males and females when it comes to how people perceive valuing health states for children. Future research investigating differences between males and females are warranted. In our study, all adolescents had experience with children and a majority of adults were parents, this might have influenced how the perceived the task of valuing health states for a child.
It is important to reflect on how the results from our study can be transferred to and valid for other settings and populations [36, 39, 40]. In our study, the context, sample strategies, sample size, participants, and procedures are described thoroughly to enable the reader to form an opinion regarding transferability [41]. However, as the EQ-5D-Y-3L is an international instrument, the questions asked in this study are most likely relevant for other contexts where the instrument is being used. To enhance credibility, a sample of the transcribed material was coded individually by first and the second author, discussed and compared. Quotes from participants increase confirmability. To promote dependability, activities and decisions were documented. Some qualitative studies have previously examined psychometric properties of the EQ-5D [4244], but this is, to the best of our knowledge, the first study investigating what lies behind the numbers when valuing health states according to the standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L. Results from the present study can increase understanding of how adolescents and adults perceive completing such valuations and encourage similar studies in the protocol development and when planning full-scale valuation studies. Furthermore, this study provides important knowledge regarding how emotional strenuous these valuation tasks were for the respondent and raises the question if this is how we want people to react when valuing health state for children. Differences between the child perspective and own perspective need to be thought of and addressed when applying similar approaches in the standardised valuation protocol for EQ-5D-Y-3L as for the valuation protocol for the adult version EQ-5D-5L.

Conclusion

Thoughts and feelings were explored, and strategies revealed. Although adolescents and adults managed to complete the valuation tasks to derive values for the EQ-5D-Y-3L, doubt, awfulness and being reluctant to trade-off life years were commonly expressed feelings. Participants questioned who has the right to value health states for someone else and recognised their own limitations in judging health states unfamiliar to them. Experience and point of view were used as strategies to complete the valuation tasks. These findings can contribute to the need of further developing the standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L as well as understanding and interpretating the quantitative results.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the adults and adolescents for participating and generously sharing their thoughts and feelings during the interviews, this study would not have been possible without you. Furthermore, discussions with Professor Ella Danielson regarding a previous version of the interview guide are gratefully acknowledged. Valuable comments on earlier versions of this paper when presented in the Health Outcomes and Economic Evaluation Research Group and in the Equity and Health Policy Research Group at Karolinska Institutet are thankfully acknowledged.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors Mimmi Åström and Kristina Burström are members of the EuroQol Group and Mimmi Åström is employed by Region Stockholm. Neither of these had any influence regarding the study design, interpretation of results or in formulating the manuscript. The authors Helen Conte and Jenny Berg have no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article. The views expressed by the authors in the publication do not necessarily reflect the views of the EuroQol Group.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee [Swedish Ethical Review Authority (Dnr: 2020-03753, 2020-05390)] and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was collected orally by recording a confirmation that participants had received the letter of information and that they consented to participate in the study. Informed consent was not collected from adolescents’ parents or guardians as, according to Swedish law, adolescents from 15 years old can give their own consent if they are able to understand and weigh the consequences of participating. All participants received a gift card, sent by to them by e-mail after the interview, as a compensation for the time they spent on participating.
Not applicable.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Podotherapeut Totaal

Binnen de bundel kunt u gebruik maken van boeken, tijdschriften, e-learnings, web-tv's en uitlegvideo's. BSL Podotherapeut Totaal is overal toegankelijk; via uw PC, tablet of smartphone.

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Rowen, D., Rivero-Arias, O., Devlin, N., & Ratcliffe, J. (2020). Review of valuation methods of preference-based measures of health for economic evaluation in child and adolescent populations: where are we now and where are we going? PharmacoEconomics, 38(4), 325–340.PubMedCrossRef Rowen, D., Rivero-Arias, O., Devlin, N., & Ratcliffe, J. (2020). Review of valuation methods of preference-based measures of health for economic evaluation in child and adolescent populations: where are we now and where are we going? PharmacoEconomics, 38(4), 325–340.PubMedCrossRef
2.
go back to reference Wille, N., Badia, X., Bonsel, G., Burström, K., Cavrini, G., Devlin, N., Egmar, A. C., Greiner, W., Gusi, N., Herdman, M., Jelsma, J., Kind, P., Scalone, L., & Ravens-Sieberer, U. (2010). Development of the EQ-5D-Y: A child-friendly version of the EQ-5D. Quality of Life Research, 19(6), 875–886.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wille, N., Badia, X., Bonsel, G., Burström, K., Cavrini, G., Devlin, N., Egmar, A. C., Greiner, W., Gusi, N., Herdman, M., Jelsma, J., Kind, P., Scalone, L., & Ravens-Sieberer, U. (2010). Development of the EQ-5D-Y: A child-friendly version of the EQ-5D. Quality of Life Research, 19(6), 875–886.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
3.
go back to reference Burström, K., Egmar, A. C., Lugnér, A., Eriksson, M., & Svartengren, M. A. (2010). Swedish child-friendly pilot version of the EQ-5D instrument—the development process. European Journal of Public Health., 21(2), 171–177.PubMedCrossRef Burström, K., Egmar, A. C., Lugnér, A., Eriksson, M., & Svartengren, M. A. (2010). Swedish child-friendly pilot version of the EQ-5D instrument—the development process. European Journal of Public Health., 21(2), 171–177.PubMedCrossRef
4.
go back to reference Brazier, J., Ratcliffe, J., Saloman, J., & Tsuchiya, A. (2007). Measuring and valuing health benefits for economic evaluation. Oxford University Press. Brazier, J., Ratcliffe, J., Saloman, J., & Tsuchiya, A. (2007). Measuring and valuing health benefits for economic evaluation. Oxford University Press.
5.
go back to reference Jiang, M., Ma, Y., Li, M., Meng, R., Ma, A., & Chen, P. (2021). A comparison of self-reported and proxy-reported health utilities in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Qual Life Outcomes, 19(1), 45.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jiang, M., Ma, Y., Li, M., Meng, R., Ma, A., & Chen, P. (2021). A comparison of self-reported and proxy-reported health utilities in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Qual Life Outcomes, 19(1), 45.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
6.
go back to reference Ramos-Goñi, J. M., Oppe, M., Stolk, E., Shah, K., Kreimeier, S., Rivero-Arias, O., & Devlin, N. (2020). International valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L. PharmacoEconomics, 38(7), 653–663.PubMedCrossRef Ramos-Goñi, J. M., Oppe, M., Stolk, E., Shah, K., Kreimeier, S., Rivero-Arias, O., & Devlin, N. (2020). International valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L. PharmacoEconomics, 38(7), 653–663.PubMedCrossRef
8.
go back to reference Shiroiwa, T., Ikeda, S., Noto, S., Fukuda, T., & Stolk, E. (2021). Valuation survey of EQ-5D-Y based on the international common protocol: Development of a value set in Japan. Medical Decision Making, 41(5), 597–606.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Shiroiwa, T., Ikeda, S., Noto, S., Fukuda, T., & Stolk, E. (2021). Valuation survey of EQ-5D-Y based on the international common protocol: Development of a value set in Japan. Medical Decision Making, 41(5), 597–606.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
9.
go back to reference Chen, G., & Ratcliffe, J. (2015). A review of the development and application of generic multi-attribute utility instruments for paediatric populations. PharmacoEconomics, 33(10), 1013–1028.PubMedCrossRef Chen, G., & Ratcliffe, J. (2015). A review of the development and application of generic multi-attribute utility instruments for paediatric populations. PharmacoEconomics, 33(10), 1013–1028.PubMedCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Kreimeier, S., & Greiner, W. (2019). EQ-5D-Y as a health-related quality of life instrument for children and adolescents: the instrument’s characteristics, development, current use, and challenges of developing its value set. Value Health, 22(1), 31–37.PubMedCrossRef Kreimeier, S., & Greiner, W. (2019). EQ-5D-Y as a health-related quality of life instrument for children and adolescents: the instrument’s characteristics, development, current use, and challenges of developing its value set. Value Health, 22(1), 31–37.PubMedCrossRef
11.
go back to reference Kreimeier, S., Oppe, M., Ramos-Goñi, J. M., Cole, A., Devlin, N., Herdman, M., Mulhern, B., Shah, K. K., Stolk, E., Rivero-Arias, O., & Greiner, W. (2018). Valuation of EuroQol five-dimensional questionnaire, youth version (EQ-5D-Y) and EuroQol five-dimensional questionnaire, three-level version (EQ-5D-3L) health states: The impact of wording and perspective. Value Health, 21(11), 1291–1298.PubMedCrossRef Kreimeier, S., Oppe, M., Ramos-Goñi, J. M., Cole, A., Devlin, N., Herdman, M., Mulhern, B., Shah, K. K., Stolk, E., Rivero-Arias, O., & Greiner, W. (2018). Valuation of EuroQol five-dimensional questionnaire, youth version (EQ-5D-Y) and EuroQol five-dimensional questionnaire, three-level version (EQ-5D-3L) health states: The impact of wording and perspective. Value Health, 21(11), 1291–1298.PubMedCrossRef
12.
go back to reference Lipman, S. A., Reckers-Droog, V. T., & Kreimeier, S. (2021). Think of the children: A discussion of the rationale for and implications of the perspective used for EQ-5D-Y health state valuation. Value Health, 24(7), 976–982.PubMedCrossRef Lipman, S. A., Reckers-Droog, V. T., & Kreimeier, S. (2021). Think of the children: A discussion of the rationale for and implications of the perspective used for EQ-5D-Y health state valuation. Value Health, 24(7), 976–982.PubMedCrossRef
13.
go back to reference Dolan, P. (1999). Valuing health-related quality of life Issues and controversies. Pharmacoeconomics, 15(2), 119–127.PubMedCrossRef Dolan, P. (1999). Valuing health-related quality of life Issues and controversies. Pharmacoeconomics, 15(2), 119–127.PubMedCrossRef
14.
go back to reference Devlin, N. J., et al. (2004). Understanding health state valuations: A qualitative analysis of respondents’ comments. Quality of Life Research., 13(7), 1265–1277.PubMedCrossRef Devlin, N. J., et al. (2004). Understanding health state valuations: A qualitative analysis of respondents’ comments. Quality of Life Research., 13(7), 1265–1277.PubMedCrossRef
15.
go back to reference Karimi, M., Brazier, J., & Paisley, S. (2017). How do individuals value health states? A qualitative investigation. Soc Sci Med, 172, 80–88.PubMedCrossRef Karimi, M., Brazier, J., & Paisley, S. (2017). How do individuals value health states? A qualitative investigation. Soc Sci Med, 172, 80–88.PubMedCrossRef
17.
go back to reference Powell, P. A., Rowen, D., Rivero-Arias, O., Tsuchiya, A., & Brazier, J. E. (2021). Valuing child and adolescent health: A qualitative study on different perspectives and priorities taken by the adult general public. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 19(1), 222.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Powell, P. A., Rowen, D., Rivero-Arias, O., Tsuchiya, A., & Brazier, J. E. (2021). Valuing child and adolescent health: A qualitative study on different perspectives and priorities taken by the adult general public. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 19(1), 222.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
go back to reference Elo, S., & Kyngäs, H. (2008). The qualitative content analysis process. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 62(1), 107–115.PubMedCrossRef Elo, S., & Kyngäs, H. (2008). The qualitative content analysis process. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 62(1), 107–115.PubMedCrossRef
19.
go back to reference Tong, A., Sainsbury, P., & Craig, J. (2007). Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): A 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. International Journal for Quality in Health Care, 19(6), 349–357.PubMedCrossRef Tong, A., Sainsbury, P., & Craig, J. (2007). Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): A 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. International Journal for Quality in Health Care, 19(6), 349–357.PubMedCrossRef
20.
go back to reference Moser, A., & Korstjens, I. (2018). Series: Practical guidance to qualitative research. Part 3: Sampling, data collection and analysis. European Journal of General Practice, 24(1), 9–18.PubMedCrossRef Moser, A., & Korstjens, I. (2018). Series: Practical guidance to qualitative research. Part 3: Sampling, data collection and analysis. European Journal of General Practice, 24(1), 9–18.PubMedCrossRef
21.
go back to reference Kreimeier, S., Åström, M., Burström, K., Egmar, A.-C., Gusi, N., Herdman, M., Kind, P., Perez-Sousa, M., & Greiner, W. (2019). EQ-5D-Y-5L: Developing a revised EQ-5D-Y with increased response categories. Quality of Life Research, 28(7), 1951–1961.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kreimeier, S., Åström, M., Burström, K., Egmar, A.-C., Gusi, N., Herdman, M., Kind, P., Perez-Sousa, M., & Greiner, W. (2019). EQ-5D-Y-5L: Developing a revised EQ-5D-Y with increased response categories. Quality of Life Research, 28(7), 1951–1961.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
22.
go back to reference Goodwin, E., Davey, A., Green, C., & Hawton, A. (2021). What drives differences in preferences for health states between patients and the public? A qualitative investigation of respondents’ thought processes. Social Science & Medicine, 282, 114150.CrossRef Goodwin, E., Davey, A., Green, C., & Hawton, A. (2021). What drives differences in preferences for health states between patients and the public? A qualitative investigation of respondents’ thought processes. Social Science & Medicine, 282, 114150.CrossRef
23.
go back to reference Mott, D. J., Shah, K. K., Ramos-Goñi, J. M., Devlin, N. J., & Rivero-Arias, O. (2021). Valuing EQ-5D-Y-3L health states using a discrete choice experiment: Do adult and adolescent preferences differ? Medical Decision Making, 41(5), 584–596.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mott, D. J., Shah, K. K., Ramos-Goñi, J. M., Devlin, N. J., & Rivero-Arias, O. (2021). Valuing EQ-5D-Y-3L health states using a discrete choice experiment: Do adult and adolescent preferences differ? Medical Decision Making, 41(5), 584–596.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
go back to reference Ratcliffe, J., Stevens, K., Flynn, T., Brazier, J., & Sawyer, M. G. (2012). Whose values in health? An empirical comparison of the application of adolescent and adult values for the CHU-9D and AQOL-6D in the Australian adolescent general population. Value Health, 15(5), 730–736.PubMedCrossRef Ratcliffe, J., Stevens, K., Flynn, T., Brazier, J., & Sawyer, M. G. (2012). Whose values in health? An empirical comparison of the application of adolescent and adult values for the CHU-9D and AQOL-6D in the Australian adolescent general population. Value Health, 15(5), 730–736.PubMedCrossRef
25.
go back to reference Kane, R. L., & Radosevich, D. M. (2011). Conducting health outcomes research. Jones and Bartlett Publishers. Kane, R. L., & Radosevich, D. M. (2011). Conducting health outcomes research. Jones and Bartlett Publishers.
26.
go back to reference Burström, K., Sun, S., Gerdtham, U. G., Henriksson, M., Johannesson, M., Levin, L. Å., & Zethraeus, N. (2014). Swedish experience-based value sets for EQ-5D health states. Quality of Life Research, 23(2), 431–442.PubMedCrossRef Burström, K., Sun, S., Gerdtham, U. G., Henriksson, M., Johannesson, M., Levin, L. Å., & Zethraeus, N. (2014). Swedish experience-based value sets for EQ-5D health states. Quality of Life Research, 23(2), 431–442.PubMedCrossRef
27.
go back to reference Burström, K., Teni, F. S., Gerdtham, U. G., Leidl, R., Helgesson, G., Rolfson, O., & Henriksson, M. (2020). Experience-based Swedish TTO and VAS value sets for EQ-5D-5L health states. PharmacoEconomics, 38(8), 839–856.PubMedCrossRef Burström, K., Teni, F. S., Gerdtham, U. G., Leidl, R., Helgesson, G., Rolfson, O., & Henriksson, M. (2020). Experience-based Swedish TTO and VAS value sets for EQ-5D-5L health states. PharmacoEconomics, 38(8), 839–856.PubMedCrossRef
28.
go back to reference Åström, M., Persson, C., Lindén-Boström, M., Rolfson, O., & Burström, K. (2018). Population health status based on the EQ-5D-Y-3L among adolescents in Sweden: Results by sociodemographic factors and self-reported comorbidity. Quality of Life Research, 27(11), 2859–2871.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Åström, M., Persson, C., Lindén-Boström, M., Rolfson, O., & Burström, K. (2018). Population health status based on the EQ-5D-Y-3L among adolescents in Sweden: Results by sociodemographic factors and self-reported comorbidity. Quality of Life Research, 27(11), 2859–2871.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
29.
go back to reference Lloyd, A. J. (2003). Threats to the estimation of benefit: Are preference elicitation methods accurate? Health Economics, 12(5), 393–402.PubMedCrossRef Lloyd, A. J. (2003). Threats to the estimation of benefit: Are preference elicitation methods accurate? Health Economics, 12(5), 393–402.PubMedCrossRef
30.
go back to reference Helgesson, G., Ernstsson, O., Åström, M., & Burström, K. (2020). Whom should we ask? A systematic literature review of the arguments regarding the most accurate source of information for valuation of health states. Quality of Life Research, 29(6), 1465–1482.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Helgesson, G., Ernstsson, O., Åström, M., & Burström, K. (2020). Whom should we ask? A systematic literature review of the arguments regarding the most accurate source of information for valuation of health states. Quality of Life Research, 29(6), 1465–1482.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
31.
go back to reference Versteegh, M. M., & Brouwer, W. B. F. (2016). Patient and general public preferences for health states: A call to reconsider current guidelines. Social Science and Medicine, 165, 66–74.PubMedCrossRef Versteegh, M. M., & Brouwer, W. B. F. (2016). Patient and general public preferences for health states: A call to reconsider current guidelines. Social Science and Medicine, 165, 66–74.PubMedCrossRef
32.
go back to reference Stolk, E., Ludwig, K., Rand, K., van Hout, B., & Ramos-Goñi, J. M. (2019). Overview, update, and lessons learned from the international EQ-5D-5L valuation work: Version 2 of the EQ-5D-5L valuation protocol. Value Health, 22(1), 23–30.PubMedCrossRef Stolk, E., Ludwig, K., Rand, K., van Hout, B., & Ramos-Goñi, J. M. (2019). Overview, update, and lessons learned from the international EQ-5D-5L valuation work: Version 2 of the EQ-5D-5L valuation protocol. Value Health, 22(1), 23–30.PubMedCrossRef
33.
go back to reference Rapkin, B. D., & Schwartz, C. E. (2004). Toward a theoretical model of quality-of-life appraisal: Implications of findings from studies of response shift. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 2, 14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rapkin, B. D., & Schwartz, C. E. (2004). Toward a theoretical model of quality-of-life appraisal: Implications of findings from studies of response shift. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 2, 14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
34.
go back to reference Ernstsson, O., Burström, K., Heintz, E., & Mølsted Alvesson, H. (2020). Reporting and valuing one’s own health: A think aloud study using EQ-5D-5L, EQ VAS and a time trade-off question among patients with a chronic condition. Health Quality Life Outcomes, 18(1), 388.CrossRef Ernstsson, O., Burström, K., Heintz, E., & Mølsted Alvesson, H. (2020). Reporting and valuing one’s own health: A think aloud study using EQ-5D-5L, EQ VAS and a time trade-off question among patients with a chronic condition. Health Quality Life Outcomes, 18(1), 388.CrossRef
35.
go back to reference Cubi-Molla, P., Shah, K., & Burström, K. (2018). Experience-based values: A framework for classifying different types of experience in health valuation research. Patient, 11(3), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Cubi-Molla, P., Shah, K., & Burström, K. (2018). Experience-based values: A framework for classifying different types of experience in health valuation research. Patient, 11(3), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
36.
go back to reference Green, J., & Thorogood, N. (2018). Qualitative methods for health research (4th ed.). SAGE Publication. Green, J., & Thorogood, N. (2018). Qualitative methods for health research (4th ed.). SAGE Publication.
37.
go back to reference Janghorban, R., Latifnejad Roudsari, R., & Taghipour, A. (2014). Skype interviewing: The new generation of online synchronous interview in qualitative research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being, 15(9), 24152.CrossRef Janghorban, R., Latifnejad Roudsari, R., & Taghipour, A. (2014). Skype interviewing: The new generation of online synchronous interview in qualitative research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being, 15(9), 24152.CrossRef
38.
go back to reference Lipman, S. A. (2020). Time for Tele-TTO? Lessons learned from digital interviewer-assisted time trade-off data collection. Patient, 21, 1–11. Lipman, S. A. (2020). Time for Tele-TTO? Lessons learned from digital interviewer-assisted time trade-off data collection. Patient, 21, 1–11.
39.
go back to reference Malterud K. (2014) Kvalitativa metoder i medicinsk forskning: en introduktion. 3., [uppdaterade] uppl. Studentlitteratur. Malterud K. (2014) Kvalitativa metoder i medicinsk forskning: en introduktion. 3., [uppdaterade] uppl. Studentlitteratur.
40.
go back to reference Coast, J., McDonald, R., & Baker, R. (2004). Issues arising from the use of qualitative methods in health economics. Journal of Health Services Research & Policy, 9(3), 171–176.CrossRef Coast, J., McDonald, R., & Baker, R. (2004). Issues arising from the use of qualitative methods in health economics. Journal of Health Services Research & Policy, 9(3), 171–176.CrossRef
41.
go back to reference Korstjens, I., & Moser, A. (2018). Series: Practical guidance to qualitative research. Part 4: Trustworthiness and publishing. European Journal of General Practice, 24(1), 120–124.PubMedCrossRef Korstjens, I., & Moser, A. (2018). Series: Practical guidance to qualitative research. Part 4: Trustworthiness and publishing. European Journal of General Practice, 24(1), 120–124.PubMedCrossRef
42.
go back to reference Tan, R. L., Yang, Z., Igarashi, A., Herdman, M., & Luo, N. (2021). How do respondents interpret and view the EQ-VAS? A qualitative study of three Asian populations. Patient, 14(2), 283–293.PubMedCrossRef Tan, R. L., Yang, Z., Igarashi, A., Herdman, M., & Luo, N. (2021). How do respondents interpret and view the EQ-VAS? A qualitative study of three Asian populations. Patient, 14(2), 283–293.PubMedCrossRef
43.
go back to reference Yang, F., Jiang, S., He, X. N., Li, H. C., Wu, H. Y., Zhang, T. T., & Wu, J. (2021). Do rural residents in China understand EQ-5D-5L as intended? Evidence from a qualitative study. Pharmacoecon Open, 5(1), 101–109.PubMedCrossRef Yang, F., Jiang, S., He, X. N., Li, H. C., Wu, H. Y., Zhang, T. T., & Wu, J. (2021). Do rural residents in China understand EQ-5D-5L as intended? Evidence from a qualitative study. Pharmacoecon Open, 5(1), 101–109.PubMedCrossRef
44.
go back to reference Krig, S., Åström, M., Kulane, A., & Burström, K. (2021). Acceptability of the health-related quality of life instrument EQ-5D-Y-5L among patients in child and adolescent psychiatric inpatient care. Acta Paediatrica, 110(3), 899–906.PubMedCrossRef Krig, S., Åström, M., Kulane, A., & Burström, K. (2021). Acceptability of the health-related quality of life instrument EQ-5D-Y-5L among patients in child and adolescent psychiatric inpatient care. Acta Paediatrica, 110(3), 899–906.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
‘Like holding the axe on who should live or not’: adolescents’ and adults’ perceptions of valuing children’s health states using a standardised valuation protocol for the EQ-5D-Y-3L
Auteurs
Mimmi Åström
Helen Conte
Jenny Berg
Kristina Burström
Publicatiedatum
24-02-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer International Publishing
Gepubliceerd in
Quality of Life Research / Uitgave 7/2022
Print ISSN: 0962-9343
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2649
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-022-03107-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2022

Quality of Life Research 7/2022 Naar de uitgave