Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2011

01-01-2011 | Original Article

Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions

Auteurs: Roland Pfister, Andrea Kiesel, Joachim Hoffmann

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Recent studies reported converging evidence for action–effect associations if participants adopted an intention-based action control mode in free choice conditions, whereas no evidence for action–effect associations was found when participants adopted a stimulus-based mode in forced choice conditions. However, it is not yet clear whether action control modes moderate acquisition or usage of action–effect associations. In the present experiment, two groups of participants underwent an acquisition phase consisting of either free or forced choice key presses that produced irrelevant, but contingent effect tones. In a subsequent test phase, participants freely chose the key to press after former effect tones were presented. A reliable consistency effect resulted for both the groups, i.e. participants preferred the key that produced the irrelevant tone in the preceding acquisition phase. In combination with prior findings, this consistency effect suggests that usage, but not acquisition of action–effect associations depends on an intention-based action control mode.
Voetnoten
1
Prior implementations of the current experimental design used two independent criteria for participant selection (Elsner & Hommel, 2001, Exp. 3B; Hoffmann et al., 2009, Exp. 2). As first selection criterion, more than 10% erroneous responses in nogo-trials were considered as an indicator of response selection prior to go-signal onset. This behaviour would lead to reduced consistency effects as a prepared response is unlikely to be affected by tones accompanying the go-signal. This tendency was not present in the current data-set (r = 0.12 between the percentage of consistent choices and the number of nogo-errors). As second selection criterion, relative percentages of left and right reactions were compared and participants were excluded from data analysis when the distribution was not considered as even. This procedure is statistically warranted as an uneven distribution of response choices inevitably reduces the chance of finding a consistency effect that might be present in the data. This trend was also present in the current data, indicated by a correlation of r = −0.15 between the relative percentage of consistent key presses and the percentage of the preferred response. However, to obtain a conservative estimate of the consistency effect, we did not exclude any participant in the present study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens [Analysis of the Will]. Berlin: Urban & Schwarzenberg. Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens [Analysis of the Will]. Berlin: Urban & Schwarzenberg.
go back to reference Dutzi, I. B., & Hommel, B. (2009). The microgenesis of action–effect binding. Psychological Research, 73, 425–435.CrossRefPubMed Dutzi, I. B., & Hommel, B. (2009). The microgenesis of action–effect binding. Psychological Research, 73, 425–435.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A choice reaction time test of ideomotor theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 20–25.CrossRefPubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A choice reaction time test of ideomotor theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 20–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik [Psychology as a science newly founded on experience, metaphysics, and mathematics]. Königsberg: August Wilhelm Unzer. Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik [Psychology as a science newly founded on experience, metaphysics, and mathematics]. Königsberg: August Wilhelm Unzer.
go back to reference Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1540–1554. Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1540–1554.
go back to reference Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2009). Intention and attention in ideomotor learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 219–227. Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2009). Intention and attention in ideomotor learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 219–227.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Berner, M., Butz, M. V., Herbort, O., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., et al. (2007). Explorations of anticipatory behavioral control (ABC): A report from the cognitive psychology unit of the University of Würzburg. Cognitive Processing, 8, 133–142.CrossRefPubMed Hoffmann, J., Berner, M., Butz, M. V., Herbort, O., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., et al. (2007). Explorations of anticipatory behavioral control (ABC): A report from the cognitive psychology unit of the University of Würzburg. Cognitive Processing, 8, 133–142.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Lenhard, A., Sebald, A., & Pfister, R. (2009). Movements or targets: What makes an action in action effect learning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 2433–2449.CrossRef Hoffmann, J., Lenhard, A., Sebald, A., & Pfister, R. (2009). Movements or targets: What makes an action in action effect learning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 2433–2449.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.CrossRefPubMed Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Alonso, D., & Fuentes, L. J. (2003). Acquisition and generalization of action effects. Visual Cognition, 10, 965–986.CrossRef Hommel, B., Alonso, D., & Fuentes, L. J. (2003). Acquisition and generalization of action effects. Visual Cognition, 10, 965–986.CrossRef
go back to reference Keller, P. E., Wascher, E., Prinz, W., Waszak, F., Koch, I., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2006). Differences between intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 20, 9–20.CrossRef Keller, P. E., Wascher, E., Prinz, W., Waszak, F., Koch, I., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2006). Differences between intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 20, 9–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Variable action effects: Response control by context-specific effect anticipations. Psychological Research, 68, 155–162.CrossRefPubMed Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Variable action effects: Response control by context-specific effect anticipations. Psychological Research, 68, 155–162.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: Evidence for non action–effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 542–549.CrossRef Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: Evidence for non action–effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 542–549.CrossRef
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response–effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.CrossRefPubMed Kunde, W. (2001). Response–effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., Elsner, K., & Kiesel, A. (2007). No anticipation-no action: The role of anticipation in action and perception. Cognitive Processing, 8, 71–78.CrossRefPubMed Kunde, W., Elsner, K., & Kiesel, A. (2007). No anticipation-no action: The role of anticipation in action and perception. Cognitive Processing, 8, 71–78.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57A, 87–106.CrossRef Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57A, 87–106.CrossRef
go back to reference Maes, J. H. (2006). Response bias induces in rats by response effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1346–1356.CrossRef Maes, J. H. (2006). Response bias induces in rats by response effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1346–1356.CrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1998). Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 10–20. Prinz, W. (1998). Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 10–20.
go back to reference Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R–O learning blocks S–R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.CrossRef Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R–O learning blocks S–R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.CrossRef
go back to reference Wenke, D., Waszak, F., & Haggard, P. (2009). Action selection and action awareness. Psychological Research, 73, 602–612.CrossRefPubMed Wenke, D., Waszak, F., & Haggard, P. (2009). Action selection and action awareness. Psychological Research, 73, 602–612.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ziessler, M. (1998). Response–effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.CrossRef Ziessler, M. (1998). Response–effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions
Auteurs
Roland Pfister
Andrea Kiesel
Joachim Hoffmann
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2011

Psychological Research 1/2011 Naar de uitgave

Editorial

Editorial