Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2010

01-07-2010 | Original Article

Late backward effects in the refractory period paradigm: effects of Task 2 execution on Task 1 performance

Auteurs: Susana Ruiz Fernández, Rolf Ulrich

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The central bottleneck model assumes that in the psychological refractory paradigm, Task 1 performance is independent of Task 2 demands. Previous studies, however, have reported backward crosstalk effects of motor demands in Task 2 on Task 1 performance. These effects have been attributed to interference at the central level. The present study aimed to isolate more directly potential backward effects at the motor level. Therefore, in three experiments, movement distance in Task 2 was manipulated using a guided ballistic movement. The results showed that movement distance in Task 2 affected reaction time as well as response duration in Task 1. It is argued that the backward effect observed in this study is due to response coupling at motor rather than central levels.
Voetnoten
1
In all experiments, the backward crosstalk effect was robust because it did not disappear when trials with grouped responses were discarded, i.e., trials with IRIs less than or equal to 100 ms. Furthermore, the effect did not change (p < 0.05) applying different IRI thresholds (i.e., IRI < 50 ms, IRI < 100 ms and IRI < 200 ms). Thus, we reject response grouping as possible explanation for the backward crosstalk effect observed in this study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (in press). The source of execution-related task-interference: Motor bottleneck or response monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance. Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (in press). The source of execution-related task-interference: Motor bottleneck or response monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance.
go back to reference Bratzke, D., Ulrich, R., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., Jentzsch, I., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 61, 1385–1399. Bratzke, D., Ulrich, R., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., Jentzsch, I., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 61, 1385–1399.
go back to reference Caessens, B., Hommel, B., Reynvoet, B., & van der Goten, K. (2004). Backward-compatibility effects with irrelevant stimulus–response overlap: The case of the SNARC effect. Journal of General Psychology, 13, 411–425. Caessens, B., Hommel, B., Reynvoet, B., & van der Goten, K. (2004). Backward-compatibility effects with irrelevant stimulus–response overlap: The case of the SNARC effect. Journal of General Psychology, 13, 411–425.
go back to reference De Jong, R. (1993). Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 965–980.CrossRef De Jong, R. (1993). Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 965–980.CrossRef
go back to reference Freund, H.-J., & Büdingen, H. J. (1978). The relationship between speed and amplitude of the fastest voluntary contractions of human arm muscles. Experimental Brain Research, 31, 1–12.CrossRef Freund, H.-J., & Büdingen, H. J. (1978). The relationship between speed and amplitude of the fastest voluntary contractions of human arm muscles. Experimental Brain Research, 31, 1–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Giray, M., & Ulrich, R. (1993). Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 1278–1291.CrossRef Giray, M., & Ulrich, R. (1993). Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 1278–1291.CrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 291–345.CrossRefPubMed Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 291–345.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1995). Models for response-response compatibility: The effects of relation between responses in a choice task. Acta Psychologica, 90, 315–332.CrossRef Heuer, H. (1995). Models for response-response compatibility: The effects of relation between responses in a choice task. Acta Psychologica, 90, 315–332.CrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1996). Coordination. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. 3: Attention (pp. 121–180). London: Academic Press. Heuer, H. (1996). Coordination. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. 3: Attention (pp. 121–180). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., Spijkers, W., & Steglich, C. (2004). Intermanual cross-talk effects in unimanual choice reactions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 57, 993–1018.PubMed Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., Spijkers, W., & Steglich, C. (2004). Intermanual cross-talk effects in unimanual choice reactions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 57, 993–1018.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 1368–1384.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 1368–1384.CrossRef
go back to reference Jentzsch, I., & Dudschig, C. (2009). Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 209–218. Jentzsch, I., & Dudschig, C. (2009). Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 209–218.
go back to reference Jentzsch, I., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Decomposing sources of response slowing in the PRP paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 610–626.CrossRef Jentzsch, I., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Decomposing sources of response slowing in the PRP paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 610–626.CrossRef
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 1015–1027.CrossRef Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 1015–1027.CrossRef
go back to reference Koch, I., & Prinz, W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 192–201.CrossRef Koch, I., & Prinz, W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 192–201.CrossRef
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus–response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238. Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus–response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., Ruthruff, E., Hsieh, S., & Yu, Y.-T. (2007). Parallel central processing between tasks: Evidence from lateralized readiness potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 133–141. Lien, M.-C., Ruthruff, E., Hsieh, S., & Yu, Y.-T. (2007). Parallel central processing between tasks: Evidence from lateralized readiness potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 133–141.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Delheimer, J. A. (2001). Parallel memory retrieval in dual task situations. II. Episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 668–685.CrossRef Logan, G. D., & Delheimer, J. A. (2001). Parallel memory retrieval in dual task situations. II. Episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 668–685.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual task situations. I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1072–1090.CrossRef Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual task situations. I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1072–1090.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 484–493.CrossRefPubMed Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 484–493.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miller, J., & Alderton, M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 149–165.CrossRef Miller, J., & Alderton, M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 149–165.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J., & Reynolds, A. (2003). The locus of redundant-targets and non-targets effects: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1126–1142.CrossRef Miller, J., & Reynolds, A. (2003). The locus of redundant-targets and non-targets effects: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1126–1142.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Bimanual response grouping in dual-task paradigms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 61, 999–1019. Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Bimanual response grouping in dual-task paradigms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 61, 999–1019.
go back to reference Mordkoff, J. T., Miller, J., & Roch, A. C. (1996). Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 22, 25–41.CrossRef Mordkoff, J. T., Miller, J., & Roch, A. C. (1996). Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 22, 25–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 13, 435–448.CrossRef Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 13, 435–448.CrossRef
go back to reference Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D. E. (1990). Does motor programming necessitate response execution? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16, 183–198.CrossRef Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D. E. (1990). Does motor programming necessitate response execution? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16, 183–198.CrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 10, 358–377.CrossRef Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 10, 358–377.CrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Christian, C. (1994). Bottlenecks in planning and producing vocal, manual, and foot responses. UCSD Center for Human Information Processing Technical Report. Pashler, H., & Christian, C. (1994). Bottlenecks in planning and producing vocal, manual, and foot responses. UCSD Center for Human Information Processing Technical Report.
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 41, 19–45. Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 41, 19–45.
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H. W., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S., & Quinn, J. T. (1979). Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review, 86, 415–451.CrossRef Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H. W., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S., & Quinn, J. T. (1979). Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review, 86, 415–451.CrossRef
go back to reference Schröter, H. (2006). Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1277–1305.CrossRef Schröter, H. (2006). Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1277–1305.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30, 566–582.CrossRef Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30, 566–582.CrossRef
go back to reference Spijkers, W., & Heuer, H. (1995). Structural constraints on the performance of symmetrical bimanual movements with different amplitudes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 48, 716–740. Spijkers, W., & Heuer, H. (1995). Structural constraints on the performance of symmetrical bimanual movements with different amplitudes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 48, 716–740.
go back to reference Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., & van der Loo, H. (1997). Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes: Specification interference as revealed by reaction time. Acta Psychologica, 96, 207–227.CrossRef Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., & van der Loo, H. (1997). Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes: Specification interference as revealed by reaction time. Acta Psychologica, 96, 207–227.CrossRef
go back to reference Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Steglich, C., & Kleinsorge, T. (2000). Specification of movement amplitudes for the left and right hands: Evidence for transient parametric coupling from overlapping-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1091–1105.CrossRef Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Steglich, C., & Kleinsorge, T. (2000). Specification of movement amplitudes for the left and right hands: Evidence for transient parametric coupling from overlapping-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1091–1105.CrossRef
go back to reference Steglich, C., Heuer, H., Spijkers, W., & Kleinsorge, T. (1999). Bimanual coupling during the specification of isometric forces. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 302–316.CrossRef Steglich, C., Heuer, H., Spijkers, W., & Kleinsorge, T. (1999). Bimanual coupling during the specification of isometric forces. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 302–316.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typing. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press. Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typing. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2008). Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 75–121.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2008). Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 75–121.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Ruiz Fernández, S., Jentzsch, I., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2006). Motor limitation in dual-task processing under ballistic movement conditions. Psychological Science, 17, 788–793.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, R., Ruiz Fernández, S., Jentzsch, I., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2006). Motor limitation in dual-task processing under ballistic movement conditions. Psychological Science, 17, 788–793.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ulrich, R., & Wing, A. M. (1991). A recruitment theory of force–time relations in the production of brief force pulses: The parallel force unit model. Psychological Review, 98, 268–294.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, R., & Wing, A. M. (1991). A recruitment theory of force–time relations in the production of brief force pulses: The parallel force unit model. Psychological Review, 98, 268–294.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19. Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.
Metagegevens
Titel
Late backward effects in the refractory period paradigm: effects of Task 2 execution on Task 1 performance
Auteurs
Susana Ruiz Fernández
Rolf Ulrich
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0260-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2010

Psychological Research 4/2010 Naar de uitgave