Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-09-2008 | Original Paper

Language Assessment and Development in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Auteurs: Rhiannon J. Luyster, Mary Beth Kadlec, Alice Carter, Helen Tager-Flusberg

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 8/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

One of the primary diagnostic criteria for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is the presence of a language delay or impairment. Children with ASD are now being identified at significantly younger ages, and prior research has consistently found that early language skills in this population are heterogeneous and an important predictor for later outcome. The goal of this study was to systematically investigate language in toddlers with ASD and to identify early correlates of receptive and expressive language in this population. The study included 164 toddlers with ASD between the ages of 18 and 33 months who were evaluated on several cognitive, language and behavioral measures. Results suggested good agreement among different measures of early language, including direct assessment and parent report measures. Significant concurrent predictors of receptive language included gestures, non-verbal cognitive ability and response to joint attention. For expressive language, the most significant predictors were non-verbal cognitive ability, gestures and imitation. These findings have important implications for intervention programs targeting this population.
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Fourth ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Fourth ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
go back to reference Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication & Cognition, 29(3–4), 393–424. Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication & Cognition, 29(3–4), 393–424.
go back to reference Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 89–126.PubMedCrossRef Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 89–126.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baldwin, D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Lawrence: Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Baldwin, D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Lawrence: Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
go back to reference Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.PubMedCrossRef Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bates, E., & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 293–310.PubMedCrossRef Bates, E., & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 293–310.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004–1019.CrossRef Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004–1019.CrossRef
go back to reference Bornstein, M., Haynes, M., & Painter, K. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367–393.PubMedCrossRef Bornstein, M., Haynes, M., & Painter, K. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367–393.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bosseler, A., & Massaro, D. (2003). Development and evaluation of a computer-animated tutor or vocabulary and language learning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 653–672.PubMedCrossRef Bosseler, A., & Massaro, D. (2003). Development and evaluation of a computer-animated tutor or vocabulary and language learning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 653–672.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Boucher, J. (2003). Language development in autism. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinilaryngology, 67S1, S159–S163.CrossRef Boucher, J. (2003). Language development in autism. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinilaryngology, 67S1, S159–S163.CrossRef
go back to reference Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and intersection in infancy. In J. D. Call, E. Galenson, & R. L. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of Infant Psychiatry (pp. 38–52). New York: Basic Books. Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and intersection in infancy. In J. D. Call, E. Galenson, & R. L. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of Infant Psychiatry (pp. 38–52). New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–166.CrossRef Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–166.CrossRef
go back to reference Carpenter, M., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders., 32(2), 91–106.CrossRef Carpenter, M., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders., 32(2), 91–106.CrossRef
go back to reference Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8(3), 253–278.CrossRef Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8(3), 253–278.CrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T. (2004). Matching preschool children with autism spectrum disorders and comparison children for language ability: Methodological challenges. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 59–64.PubMedCrossRef Charman, T. (2004). Matching preschool children with autism spectrum disorders and comparison children for language ability: Methodological challenges. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 59–64.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T. (2006). Imitation and the development of language. In S. Rogers & J. Willams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 96–117). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Charman, T. (2006). Imitation and the development of language. In S. Rogers & J. Willams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 96–117). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–18.CrossRef Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–18.CrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285.PubMedCrossRef Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in pre-school children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur communicative development inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213–236.PubMedCrossRef Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in pre-school children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur communicative development inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213–236.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J., & Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(5), 500–513.CrossRef Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J., & Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(5), 500–513.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349–358.PubMedCrossRef Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349–358.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.PubMedCrossRef Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference DeGiacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 131–136.CrossRef DeGiacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 131–136.CrossRef
go back to reference Delgado, C., Mundy, P., Crowson, M., Markus, J., Yale, M., & Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development: A comparison of target locations. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45, 715–719.CrossRef Delgado, C., Mundy, P., Crowson, M., Markus, J., Yale, M., & Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development: A comparison of target locations. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45, 715–719.CrossRef
go back to reference Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Dyck, M. J., Piek, J. P., Hay, D., Smith, L., & Hallmayer, J. (2006). Are abilities abnormally interdependent in children with autism? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35(1), 20–33.PubMedCrossRef Dyck, M. J., Piek, J. P., Hay, D., Smith, L., & Hallmayer, J. (2006). Are abilities abnormally interdependent in children with autism? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35(1), 20–33.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Ong, B., Gould, J., & Welham, M. (1998). Delayed language onset as a predictor of clinical symptoms in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 527–533.PubMedCrossRef Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Ong, B., Gould, J., & Welham, M. (1998). Delayed language onset as a predictor of clinical symptoms in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 527–533.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1–173.PubMedCrossRef Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1–173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
go back to reference Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. (2003). MacArthur communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. (2003). MacArthur communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
go back to reference Floel, A., Ellger, T., Breitenstein, C., & Knecht, S. (2003). Language perception activates the hand motor cortex: Implications for motor theories of speech perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 704–708.PubMedCrossRef Floel, A., Ellger, T., Breitenstein, C., & Knecht, S. (2003). Language perception activates the hand motor cortex: Implications for motor theories of speech perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 704–708.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ghaziuddin, M., Butler, E., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1994). Is clumsiness a marker for Asperger syndrome? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 38(5), 519–527.PubMedCrossRef Ghaziuddin, M., Butler, E., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1994). Is clumsiness a marker for Asperger syndrome? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 38(5), 519–527.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Beyond words: The importance of gesture to researchers and learners. Child Development, 71(1), 231–239.PubMedCrossRef Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Beyond words: The importance of gesture to researchers and learners. Child Development, 71(1), 231–239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(3), 367–371.PubMedCrossRef Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(3), 367–371.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 287–308.PubMedCrossRef Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 287–308.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Smith, A. (1997). Variables related to differences in standardized test outcomes for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 233–243.PubMedCrossRef Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Smith, A. (1997). Variables related to differences in standardized test outcomes for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 233–243.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laakso, M.-L., Poikkeus, A.-M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, P. (2000). Social interactional behaviors and symbolic play competence as predictors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(4), 541–556.CrossRef Laakso, M.-L., Poikkeus, A.-M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, P. (2000). Social interactional behaviors and symbolic play competence as predictors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(4), 541–556.CrossRef
go back to reference Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “Theory of Mind”. Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426.CrossRef Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “Theory of Mind”. Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from two to nine. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 694–701.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from two to nine. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 694–701.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule––Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.CrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule––Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 7–29). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 7–29). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). The autism diagnostic interview–Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.CrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). The autism diagnostic interview–Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Spence, S. J., Moldin, S. O., & Rubenstein, J. L. R. (2006). Autism spectrum disorders: Phenotype and diagnosis. In Understanding autism: From basic neuroscience to treatment (pp. 1–23). Boca Raton, FL, US: CRC Press. Lord, C., Spence, S. J., Moldin, S. O., & Rubenstein, J. L. R. (2006). Autism spectrum disorders: Phenotype and diagnosis. In Understanding autism: From basic neuroscience to treatment (pp. 1–23). Boca Raton, FL, US: CRC Press.
go back to reference Lord, C., & Ventner, A. (1992). Outcome and follow-up studies of high-functioning autistic individuals. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum. Lord, C., & Ventner, A. (1992). Outcome and follow-up studies of high-functioning autistic individuals. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum.
go back to reference Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.PubMedCrossRef Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lyytinen, P., Ahonen, T., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T., Laakso, M.-L., Leinonen, S., Leppanen, P., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Puolakanaho, A., Richardson, U., & Viholainen, H. (2001). Developmental pathways of chidlren with and without familial risk for dyslexia during the first years of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(2), 535–554.PubMedCrossRef Lyytinen, P., Ahonen, T., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T., Laakso, M.-L., Leinonen, S., Leppanen, P., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Puolakanaho, A., Richardson, U., & Viholainen, H. (2001). Developmental pathways of chidlren with and without familial risk for dyslexia during the first years of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(2), 535–554.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405–426.CrossRef Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405–426.CrossRef
go back to reference McDuffie, A., Yoder, P., & Stone, W. (2005). Prelinguistic predictors of vocabulary in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(5), 1080–1097.PubMedCrossRef McDuffie, A., Yoder, P., & Stone, W. (2005). Prelinguistic predictors of vocabulary in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(5), 1080–1097.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McEwen, F., Happé, F., Bolton, P., Rijsdijk, F., Ronald, A., Dworzynski, K., & Plomin, R. (2007). Origins of individual differences in imitation: Links with language, pretend play, and socially insightful behavior in two-year-old twins. Child Development, 78(2), 474–492.PubMedCrossRef McEwen, F., Happé, F., Bolton, P., Rijsdijk, F., Ronald, A., Dworzynski, K., & Plomin, R. (2007). Origins of individual differences in imitation: Links with language, pretend play, and socially insightful behavior in two-year-old twins. Child Development, 78(2), 474–492.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meister, I. G., Boroojerdi, B., Foltys, H., Sparing, R., Huber, W., & Topper, R. (2003). Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language. Neuropsychologia, 41, 401–406.PubMedCrossRef Meister, I. G., Boroojerdi, B., Foltys, H., Sparing, R., Huber, W., & Topper, R. (2003). Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language. Neuropsychologia, 41, 401–406.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Yale, M., Neal, A. R., & Schwartz, H. (2000). Gaze following, temperament and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 231–236.CrossRef Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Yale, M., Neal, A. R., & Schwartz, H. (2000). Gaze following, temperament and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 231–236.CrossRef
go back to reference Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc. Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.
go back to reference Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged early social communication scales (ESCS). Available through the university of Miami Psychology Department, Coral Gables, FL. Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged early social communication scales (ESCS). Available through the university of Miami Psychology Department, Coral Gables, FL.
go back to reference Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1996). Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami. Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1996). Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.
go back to reference Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.CrossRef Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.CrossRef
go back to reference Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Nonverbal communication, developmental level and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Nonverbal communication, developmental level and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.
go back to reference Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders., 17(3), 349–364.CrossRef Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders., 17(3), 349–364.CrossRef
go back to reference Nagy, E. (2006). From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates. Infant and Child Development, 15(3), 223–232.CrossRef Nagy, E. (2006). From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates. Infant and Child Development, 15(3), 223–232.CrossRef
go back to reference Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F., & Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 219–225.CrossRef Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F., & Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 219–225.CrossRef
go back to reference Paul, R., & Fountain, R. (1999). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language delay. Infant-Toddler Intervention, 9(2), 123–135. Paul, R., & Fountain, R. (1999). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language delay. Infant-Toddler Intervention, 9(2), 123–135.
go back to reference Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool children with inadequate communication. London: MacKeith Press. Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool children with inadequate communication. London: MacKeith Press.
go back to reference Risi, S., Lord, C., Gotham, K., Corsello, C., Chrysler, C., Szatmari, P., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., & Pickles, A. (2006). Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(9), 1094.CrossRef Risi, S., Lord, C., Gotham, K., Corsello, C., Chrysler, C., Szatmari, P., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., & Pickles, A. (2006). Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(9), 1094.CrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, S., Charman, T., & Stone, W. (2006). Evidence-based interventions for language development in young children with autism. New York: Guilford Press. Rogers, S., Charman, T., & Stone, W. (2006). Evidence-based interventions for language development in young children with autism. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Rogers, S., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781.CrossRef Rogers, S., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781.CrossRef
go back to reference Seal, B., & Bonvillian, J. (1997). Sign language and motor functioning in students with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 437–466.PubMedCrossRef Seal, B., & Bonvillian, J. (1997). Sign language and motor functioning in students with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 437–466.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sigman, M. (1998). The Emanuel Miller memorial lecture 1997: Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19(6), 817–827.CrossRef Sigman, M. (1998). The Emanuel Miller memorial lecture 1997: Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19(6), 817–827.CrossRef
go back to reference Sigman, M., & McGovern, C. (2005). Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 15–23.PubMedCrossRef Sigman, M., & McGovern, C. (2005). Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 15–23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.PubMedCrossRef Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
go back to reference Stone, W., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What’s the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(6), 475–485.PubMedCrossRef Stone, W., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What’s the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(6), 475–485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stone, W., & Yoder, P. J. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 5(4), 341–361.PubMedCrossRef Stone, W., & Yoder, P. J. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 5(4), 341–361.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (submitted). Observational and parent report measures of social communication skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (submitted). Observational and parent report measures of social communication skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
go back to reference Thurm, A., Lord, C., Lee, L., & Newschaffer, C. (2007). Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1731–1734.CrossRef Thurm, A., Lord, C., Lee, L., & Newschaffer, C. (2007). Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1731–1734.CrossRef
go back to reference Tomasello, M., & Mervis, C. B. (1994). The instrument is great, but measuring comprehension is still a problem. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 174–179.CrossRef Tomasello, M., & Mervis, C. B. (1994). The instrument is great, but measuring comprehension is still a problem. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 174–179.CrossRef
go back to reference Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(8), 993–1005.PubMedCrossRef Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(8), 993–1005.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1984). The relation of play and sensorimotor behavior to language in the second year. Child Development, 55, 1448–1455.PubMedCrossRef Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1984). The relation of play and sensorimotor behavior to language in the second year. Child Development, 55, 1448–1455.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., Meyer, J. A., Neal, A. R., & Pomares, Y. B. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69.CrossRef Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., Meyer, J. A., Neal, A. R., & Pomares, Y. B. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69.CrossRef
go back to reference Watt, N., Wetherby, A., & Shumway, S. (2006). Prelinguistic predictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1224–1237.PubMedCrossRef Watt, N., Wetherby, A., & Shumway, S. (2006). Prelinguistic predictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1224–1237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Webster, R. I., & Shevell, M. I. (2004). Neurobiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Neurology, 19(7), 471–481.PubMedCrossRef Webster, R. I., & Shevell, M. I. (2004). Neurobiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Neurology, 19(7), 471–481.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493.PubMedCrossRef Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152.PubMedCrossRef Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Language Assessment and Development in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders
Auteurs
Rhiannon J. Luyster
Mary Beth Kadlec
Alice Carter
Helen Tager-Flusberg
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 8/2008
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1