Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2017

25-04-2016 | Original Article

Investigating the characteristics of “not responding”: backward crosstalk in the PRP paradigm with forced vs. free no-go decisions

Auteurs: Eva Röttger, Hilde Haider

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Have you ever thought about what it means not to act? Basically, most people think about nonactions (or “not responding”) as depending on the existence of a pre-activated response which is then inhibited. The main problem when investigating the characteristics of such no-go responses is that they do not provide reaction times. Importantly, Miller (Psychol Res 70:484–493, 2006) recently showed that in a dual-task paradigm, forced go/no-go decisions in the secondary task lead to a backward crosstalk effect (BCE) in the reaction times of the primary task. Based on this experimental setup, we conducted three experiments to investigate the characteristics of “not responding.” The goal of Experiments 1 and 2 was to compare forced-choice and free-choice no-go responses. In both experiments, we only found a BCE when participants were forced not to respond. We interpret these findings as a first hint that the BCE is not due to an active inhibition of a pre-activated response tendency. Rather, we assume that it is caused by an automatic activation of specific response features when merely perceiving the secondary stimulus (Hommel, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:1368–1384, 1998). In the forced-choice condition, the stimulus unambiguously announces a no-go response. By contrast, this is not the case in the free-choice condition as here the stimulus only signals participants to freely decide to “go” or “not to go.” Therefore, we tested in Experiment 3 rather directly if merely perceiving a stimulus unambiguously announcing a “no-go” causes a BCE. The results confirmed this. Overall, our results suggest that no-go responses do not differ conceptually from go responses.
Voetnoten
1
In the forced-choice condition, 0.1 % of all trials were classified as “short-IRI” trials, 2.9 % as RT1 outliers, and 0.0 % as RT2 outliers. 5.3 % of the trials were excluded due to errors in T1, 2.2 % due to errors in T2. In the free-choice condition 7.8 % of all trials were classified as “short-IRI” trials, 1.2 % as RT1 outliers, and 3.5 % as RT2 outliers. 4.1 % of the trials were excluded due to errors in T1, 0.0 % due to errors in T2 (as it was not possible to commit errors in the free-choice condition). Some trials were excluded due to more than one criterion.
 
2
In the forced-choice condition, one participant was excluded because in 55 % of the trials (s)he responded erroneously to S1. In the free-choice condition, two participants were excluded due to their very high “short-IRI” rate (overall 99 %/83 %) which led additionally to missing values in several cells of the design. One participant was excluded because (s)he chose the no-go response only in 15 % of all trials and a fourth participant because her/his mean IRIs exceeded the condition mean by more than 2 SDs.
 
3
In detail, 4.7 % of all trials were classified as “short-IRI” trials, 1.3 % as RT1 outliers, and 0.1 % as RT2 outliers. 2.6 % of the trials were excluded due to errors in T1, 3.0 % due to errors in T2. Some trials were excluded due to more than one criterion.
 
4
Four participants were excluded because in the free-choice trials they chose one of the two R2 options (go/no-go) in less than 10 % of all trials. This led to missing values in one or more cells of the design. Three participants were excluded mainly due to their very high “short-IRI” rate (ranging from 52 % in forced-choice trials up to 89 % in free-choice trials) which (for two participants) led to missing values in several cells as well. Another two participants were excluded because their mean IRIs (up to overall 850 ms) exceeded the condition mean by more than 2 SDs.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a Richer Model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e55–e68.CrossRefPubMed Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a Richer Model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e55–e68.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Coles, M. H., & Logan, G. D. (1995). Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 498–511.PubMed De Jong, R., Coles, M. H., & Logan, G. D. (1995). Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 498–511.PubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Coles, M. G., Logan, G. D., & Gratton, G. (1990). In search of the point of no return: The control of response processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 164–182.PubMed De Jong, R., Coles, M. G., Logan, G. D., & Gratton, G. (1990). In search of the point of no return: The control of response processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 164–182.PubMed
go back to reference Donders, F. C. (1868/1969). On the speed of mental processes (original work published 1868). Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431. Donders, F. C. (1868/1969). On the speed of mental processes (original work published 1868). Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431.
go back to reference Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., & Perea, M. (2007). A model of the go/no-go task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 389–413.CrossRef Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., & Perea, M. (2007). A model of the go/no-go task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 389–413.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMed Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Eglau, B. (2002). Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 66(4), 260–273.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., & Eglau, B. (2002). Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 66(4), 260–273.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Dambacher, M., Bieleke, M., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015a). The benefit of no choice: Goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing. Psychological Research, 79(2), 206–220.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Dambacher, M., Bieleke, M., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015a). The benefit of no choice: Goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing. Psychological Research, 79(2), 206–220.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Nolden, S., & Jolicoeur, P. (2015b). No differences in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks. Psychological Research, 79(3), 463–477.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Nolden, S., & Jolicoeur, P. (2015b). No differences in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks. Psychological Research, 79(3), 463–477.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132(1), 30–43.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132(1), 30–43.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(3), 542–549.CrossRefPubMed Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(3), 542–549.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 387–394.PubMed Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 387–394.PubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88(2), 223–242.CrossRefPubMed Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88(2), 223–242.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(4), 1260–1280.PubMed Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(4), 1260–1280.PubMed
go back to reference Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1(4), 476–490.CrossRefPubMed Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1(4), 476–490.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego: Academic Press. Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91(3), 295–327.CrossRef Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91(3), 295–327.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Delheimer, J. A. (2001). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: II. Episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 668–685.CrossRefPubMed Logan, G. D., & Delheimer, J. A. (2001). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: II. Episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 668–685.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1072–1090.PubMed Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1072–1090.PubMed
go back to reference Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effect in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 484–493.CrossRefPubMed Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effect in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 484–493.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miller, J., & Durst, M. (2014). “Just do it when you get a chance”: The effects of a background task on primary task performance. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(8), 2560–2574.CrossRef Miller, J., & Durst, M. (2014). “Just do it when you get a chance”: The effects of a background task on primary task performance. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(8), 2560–2574.CrossRef
go back to reference Paelecke, M., & Kunde, W. (2007). Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 627–644.PubMed Paelecke, M., & Kunde, W. (2007). Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 627–644.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottle-neck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 358–377.PubMed Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottle-neck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 358–377.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1992). Why don’t we perceive our brain states? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 1–20.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1992). Why don’t we perceive our brain states? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 1–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14(1), 1–36.CrossRef Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14(1), 1–36.CrossRef
go back to reference Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: Associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 649–672.CrossRef Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: Associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 649–672.CrossRef
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43(1), 2–19. Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43(1), 2–19.
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 593–602.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 593–602.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Investigating the characteristics of “not responding”: backward crosstalk in the PRP paradigm with forced vs. free no-go decisions
Auteurs
Eva Röttger
Hilde Haider
Publicatiedatum
25-04-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0772-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2017

Psychological Research 3/2017 Naar de uitgave