Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 1/2022

Open Access 04-11-2021

Interaction of Psychopathic Traits Dimensions in the Prediction of Psychological and Physical Child-to-Parent Violence in Adolescents

Auteurs: Joana Del Hoyo-Bilbao, Izaskun Orue, Esther Calvete

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment | Uitgave 1/2022

Abstract

This study examined the longitudinal relationships between three psychopathic trait dimensions (callous-unemotional, CU; grandiose-manipulative, GM; and impulsive-irresponsible, II) and their interaction in the prediction of psychological and physical child-to-parent violence (CPV). Furthermore, the study examined whether the predictive relationships were different for boys and girls. A total of 765 adolescents (463 girls), with a mean age of 15.28 years (SD = 1.04), completed measures of the psychopathic trait dimensions (CU, GM, and II) and psychological and physical CPV at the beginning of the study and six months later. At the cross-sectional level, all the psychopathic trait dimensions were related to psychological and physical CPV, although the effect sizes were small. At the longitudinal level, II predicted increased psychological CPV. A significant three-way interaction between the three psychopathic trait dimensions (CU, GM, and II) indicated that the longitudinal association between II and physical CPV was higher for adolescents with high CU and low GM. However, the predictive model did not differ for boys and girls.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10862-021-09940-w.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Child-to-parent violence (CPV) is a type of family violence characterized by repeated physical, psychological (verbal or non-verbal), or economic violence directed toward one’s parents or those who fill parenting roles (Pereira et al., 2017). According to epidemiological research, the prevalence of CPV is relatively high, especially for psychological violence. One study reported rates of 14.2% and 3.2% for repeated psychological and physical CPV last year, respectively, in a community sample of 2672 Spanish adolescents (Calvete et al., 2013). Moreover, the prevalence of CPV is generally higher among offender and clinical samples (Ibabe & Jaureguizar, 2010; Purcell et al., 2014).
To develop adequate preventive interventions, it is necessary to identify the factors that lead to CPV. Previous studies have suggested that multiple factors are involved in the development and continued perpetration of CPV, including substance use (Calvete et al., 2012), family characteristics (Ibabe & Bentler, 2016), and exposure to violence (Boxer et al., 2009; Calvete et al., 2015b). In a recent review study, the authors concluded that CPV often appears to be part of a pattern of antisocial or aggressive behavior, in which adolescents who are aggressive toward their parents tend to display aggressive behavior in general (Simmons et al., 2018). However, they also highlighted that there is limited information on the role of individual characteristics, such as cognitive and emotional factors, relevant in general aggressive behavior regarding CPV.
Psychopathic traits have been proposed as important dispositional characteristics associated with aggressive behavior. Recently, the study of psychopathic traits has been extended to the child and adolescent population (for a review, see Salekin, 2017) to explore how these personality traits develop and analyze the relationships they can have with aggressive behavior. Findings from different studies indicated that psychopathic structure in childhood and adolescence is very similar to that in adults. Further, more recent studies have suggested that psychopathic structure is organized in a continuum rather than in categories representing the presence or absence of psychopathy (Marcus et al., 2004; Salekin, 2017).
Some studies have used factor analysis to examine the dimensionality of psychopathy in children and adolescents (Andershed et al., 2002; Frick et al., 2000; Van Baardewijk et al., 2010) and proposed that psychopathic personality traits among children and adolescents comprise three interrelated dimensions: affective, interpersonal, and behavioral. The interpersonal dimension is characterized by grandiosity, superficial charm, and egocentricity (grandiose-manipulative trait dimension; GM); the affective dimension is represented by remorselessness, callousness, and lack of empathy (callous-unemotional trait dimension; CU); and the behavioral dimension implies impulsiveness and irresponsibility (impulsiveness-irresponsibility trait dimension; II). Studies have consistently found a relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and externalizing behaviors, such as antisocial and delinquent behavior (Penney & Moretti, 2007; Somma et al., 2018), overt and relational aggression (Orue et al., 2016), bullying and cyberbullying (López-Larrañaga & Orue, 2019; Orue & Calvete, 2019), drug use (Hillege et al., 2010), or intimate partner violence (Kiire, 2017).
Until recently, studies have pointed to CU as the most powerful psychopathic dimension for explaining conduct problems at all ages, including children and adolescents (Frick & White, 2008; Frick et al., 2005). However, several studies have recently highlighted that studying only CU to explain conduct problems is insufficient (Colins et al., 2018; Salekin 2016a, b2017). Thus, some studies have stressed the importance of studying the interaction between the three psychopathic dimensions to predict externalizing problems to better understand adolescents with conduct problems (Lilienfeld, 2018; Salekin, 2017). For example, Colins et al. (2018) compared two models of psychopathy to predict future and stable conduct problems. The first model included only the CU trait dimension, whereas the second was a multidimensional model that included three psychopathic trait dimensions (CU, II, and GM). The results indicated that the subtyping approach using various psychopathic traits dimensions was more informative in predicting future and stable conduct problems among children and adolescents. Moreover, in another study, Somma et al. (2018) found that the interaction of the three-trait dimensions improved the prediction of delinquent behavior in a sample of adolescents beyond the information provided by a single underlying psychopathic dimension. Similarly, Colins et al. (2014) found that the interaction of the psychopathic trait dimensions was more useful in predicting the occurrence of behavioral problems than evaluating only one of them. Specifically, they found that combining high scores in the three dimensions better predicted the occurrence of behavioral problems. Further, Fanti et al. (2018) found that combining high initial levels of conduct disorder symptoms and the CU trait dimension—as well as combining conduct disorder symptoms, GM, and II—can predict later clinically significant conduct disorder symptoms. Finally, the interaction between the psychopathic trait dimensions has also been shown to be significant in predicting bullying and cyberbullying behavior (López-Larrañaga & Orue, 2019; Orue & Andershed, 2015).
Although the association between the psychopathic trait dimensions and CPV has not specifically been examined, some previous findings have suggested that these trait dimensions could be related to this type of aggressive behavior. In addition, some cross-sectional studies have found a positive relationship between impulsivity and CPV (Calvete et al., 2011; Del Hoyo-Bilbao et al., 2020; Rico et al., 2017). In analyzing the personal files of young offenders who committed CPV, Ibabe and Jaureguizar (2010) found a lack of empathy in these adolescents. Calvete et al. (2015b) found that narcissism predicted CPV. Additionally, other types of aggressive behaviors are commonly present among adolescents who enact CPV (Pagani et al., 2004, 2009; Simmons et al., 2018). As a result, psychopathic trait dimensions could have a role in CPV.

Sex Differences in CPV and Psychopathic Trait Dimensions

The differences between girls and boys in the perpetration of CPV are not clear. Some studies conducted in community and forensic samples did not find significant differences between boys and girls (Del Hoyo-Bilbao et al., 2018b; Gámez-Guadix & Calvete, 2012; Pagani et al., 2004, 2009). However, other studies’ results suggest that, in community samples, girls tend to commit more psychological aggression than boys (Calvete & Orue, 2016; Lyons et al., 2015; Margolin & Baucom, 2014). Furthermore, some studies have indicated that, in judicial contexts, boys perpetrate higher levels of physical CPV (Boxer et al., 2009; Ghanizadeh & Jafari, 2010; Routt & Anderson, 2011), whereas, in community samples, girls perpetrate more physical CPV than boys (Calvete et al., 2013, 2015b). However, other studies found no differences in physical aggression between boys and girls in judicial samples and found that girls show higher levels of relational aggression (Stickle et al., 2012) and physical CPV (Strom et al., 2014). There is a greater consensus on sex differences in psychopathic traits. Several studies have indicated higher scores among adolescent boys in GM and CU trait dimensions (Andershed et al., 2018; Fanti & Kimonis, 2012; Fanti et al., 2009). However, in the II trait dimension, some studies have found higher scores in boys (Andershed et al., 2018; Colins et al., 2018; Fanti et al., 2009), whereas others have reported higher scores in girls (Orue et al., 2016). There are no studies on sex differences in the associations between psychopathic trait dimensions and CPV. Nevertheless, some studies have examined sex differences in the relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and other types of aggressive behavior. Furthermore, some studies have indicated the absence of sex differences in the associations between psychopathic traits and aggression and delinquency (Marsee et al., 2005; Penney & Moretti, 2007). However, other studies found sex differences when considering different modalities of aggressive behavior. For example, in a study with adolescents, the CU trait dimension predicted proactive aggression only in girls, whereas the GM trait dimension predicted proactive aggression only in boys (Orue et al., 2016).

The Present Study

It is important to identify individual variables related to CPV development (Calvete et al., 2012; Simmons et al., 2018). The similarity between psychopathic trait dimensions and some individual characteristics of adolescents who commit CPV indicates a possible relationship between II, GM, and CU trait dimensions and CPV. Thus, this study first aim to analyze the longitudinal relationships between psychopathic trait dimensions (II, GM, CU) and psychological and physical CPV perpetration. Furthermore, according to previous studies, the interaction of psychopathic trait dimensions can provide more useful information than the isolated role of each trait dimension (Colins et al., 2014; Lilienfeld, 2018). Therefore, the second purpose of this study is to examine the interaction of the psychopathic trait dimensions in predicting psychological and physical CPV. Finally, given the sex differences found in previous studies on CPV, the psychopathic trait dimensions, and the relationships between the psychopathic trait dimensions and aggression, the third objective is to examine whether the predictive model is similar in boys and girls.

Method

Participants

The initial sample comprised 1002 high school students from 13 randomly selected schools in Bizkaia (Spain). The final sample comprised 765 adolescents (463 girls and 302 boys) who completed measures at both Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2). All the participants completed the instrument at T1 and T2 six months apart. There were differences in some of the study variable means at T1 between the adolescents who completed both waves and those who failed to complete the study. Specifically, the means of adolescents who failed to complete the study were higher in CU 3.29 (SD = 3.60) vs. 2.77 (SD = 2.92); t(341.38) = 2.05, p = 0.041; II 6.63 (SD = 4.02) vs. 5.97 (SD = 3.40); t(350.76) = 2.30, p = 0.022) and physical CPV 0.08 (SD = 0.29) vs. 0.04 (SD = 0.16); t(280.72) = 2.04, p = 0.043. Participants were between 14 and 19 years old (Mage = 15.28 years, SD = 1.04), and 91.2% were Spanish, 6.2% were Latin American, 0.7% were African, 0.6% were Eastern European, and 1.3% came from other regions. To evaluate participants’ socioeconomic levels, the criteria recommended by the Spanish Society of Epidemiology (2000) were used, and the distribution was as follows: 17.1% low, 27.6% medium–low, 27.1% medium, 14.6% medium–high, and 13.7% high.

Measures

Child-to-Parent Violence

CPV was measured using the Child-to-Parent Aggression Questionnaire (CPAQ; Calvete et al., 2013). The CPAQ comprises 20 items, 10 referring to fathers and 10 referring to mothers. Of the 10 items relevant to each parent, three assess physical aggression and seven assess psychological aggression. Participants reported how often they performed each aggressive act against their parents using a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (six or more times). The CPAQ has previously shown excellent psychometric properties (Calvete & Orue, 2016). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were 0.62 and 0.73 for physical CPV and 0.86 and 0.88 for psychological CPV at T1 and T2, respectively.

Psychopathic Traits Dimensions

The Youth Psychopathic Inventory-Short Form (YPI-S; Van Baardewijk et al., 2010) was used to measure psychopathic trait dimensions. The scale evaluates three psychopathic dimensions with six items per dimension: the GM, CU, and II trait dimensions. The items were rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 4 (applies very well). The Spanish version was used in this study, and it showed good psychometric properties as well (Orue & Andershed, 2015). Cronbach’s alphas were 0.82, 0.69, and 0.71 for GM, CU, and II, respectively.

Procedure

The schools used in this study were chosen at random. First, permission was obtained from the schools. Second, all parents were informed of the nature of the study and its voluntary, anonymous, and confidential nature through an informed consent form. They were informed that their children’s participation in the study would not cause them any harm. Third, the researchers’ contact information was provided for any clarification or questions they might have. Finally, they were invited to refuse their children’s participation through the informed consent form. Only two parents refused to allow their children to be included in the study. Participants received the same information as their parents, and all adolescents consented to participate in the study. Data were collected on two occasions with a 6-month interval. To preserve each participants’ identity, codes were used to pair the questionnaires from T1 and T2. All data were recorded under the code of ethics in research and kept confidential. Preacher and Coffman’s (2006) calculator was used to perform a power analysis. The power in the study was 99.58% for a sample of 765 participants. The study and its consent procedure were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University [masked].

Data Analysis

To assess the longitudinal relationships between the psychopathic dimensions and CPV, a paths analysis was conducted via the robust maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method with the Satorra-Bentler scale chi-square (S-B χ2) with LISREL 8.8 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2006). The hypothesized model included GM, CU, and II measures at T1 and psychological and physical CPV at T1 and T2. The model included autoregressive paths between psychological and physical CPV at T1 and T2. The model also included paths from the interaction terms among the psychopathic dimensions (GM × CU, GM × II, and CU × II) and the three-way interaction term among the three psychopathic dimensions (GM × CU × II). The study followed standard procedures to create the moderation terms and transformed all the psychopathic trait dimensions into z-scores. The procedures proposed by Dawson (2018) were followed to examine the differences between previously plotted slopes once the interaction terms were significant. To examine whether the data adjusted adequately to the model, the following criteria were used to assess goodness-of-fit: CFI values of 0.95 or greater, NNFI values of 0.90 or greater, and RMSEA values of 0.08 or less indicated that the model fits the data adequately (Byrne, 2006).

Results

Descriptive Analyses and Sex Differences

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics (mean and SD) and Pearson correlations for the study variables. All correlations were significant except the correlation between CU and psychological CPV at T2 (p = 0.08). However, CU and psychological CPV at T1 were positively statistically associated, but the correlation was small. All other psychopathic dimensions were positively associated with psychological and physical CPV at T1 and T2. All correlations were small, except for the correlations between II and psychological CPV at T1 and T2, which were moderate. The psychopathic trait dimensions were also positively related, indicating that high levels in one dimension were related to high levels in the other dimensions. The correlations between trait dimensions were moderate, except for the correlation between II and CU, which was small. Psychological and physical CPV at T1 and T2 were positively and moderately related, except for psychological CPV at T1 and T2, which was highly correlated. This result indicates the stability of the CPV measurement. Table 2 presents sex differences for all the study variables. Girls obtained higher scores on the II trait dimension (d = 0.24) and psychological (d = 0.61) and physical CPV (d = 0.20) at T1 and on psychological CPV at T2 (d = 0.54). Boys obtained higher scores on CU (d = 0.38) and GM trait dimensions (d = 0.23). There were no differences between girls and boys in physical CPV at T2 (d = 0.08). Therefore, the effect sizes evaluated with Cohen’s d metric were small for all differences, except for differences in psychological CPV at both T1 and T2, which were found to be medium.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics and correlations among the study variables
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
1. Callous-Unemotional
1
      
2. Grandiose-Manipulative
.38***
1
     
3. Impulsive-Irresponsible
.28***
.37***
1
    
4. Psychological CPV T1
.10***
.21***
.40***
1
   
5. Physical CPV T1
.08*
.09**
.18***
.40***
1
  
6. Psychological CPV T2
.06
.19***
.40***
.74***
.35***
1
 
7. Physical CPV T2
.07*
.08*
.15***
.34***
.47***
.36***
1
M
2.77
3.26
5.98
0.56
0.38
0.56
0.03
SD
2.95
3.60
3.41
0.49
0.16
0.47
0.13
* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001
Table 2
Sex Differences for all variables of the study
 
Girls (N = 463)
Boys (N = 302)
   
 
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
t
p
Cohen´s d
Callous-Unemotional
2.33
2.73
3.45
3.14
-5.08
.000
0.38
Grandiose-Manipulative
2.92
3.22
3.78
4.07
-3.12
.002
0.23
Impulsive-Irresponsible
6.30
3.37
5.47
3.43
3.28
.001
0.24
Psychological CPV T1
0.67
0.52
0.39
0.38
8.45
.000
0.61
Physical CPV T1
0.05
0.19
0.02
0.09
3.24
.001
0.20
Psychological CPV T2
0.66
0.49
0.42
0.40
7.46
.000
0.54
Physical CPV T2
0.03
0.15
0.02
0.09
1.32
.187
0.08

Predictive Model

First, the hypothesized path analysis model was estimated, including all the relationships between the study variables, and the fit indices were: S−Bχ2 (12, N = 765) = 102.06, RMSEA = 0.099, 90% CI [0.082, 0.12], CFI = 0.97, NNFI = 0.87. Second, the nonsignificant paths were eliminated to create a more parsimonious model. The parsimonious predictive model (Fig. 1) obtained adequate fit indices: S−Bχ2 (31, N = 765) = 161.85, RMSEA = 0.074, 90% CI [0.063, 0.086], CFI = 0.96, NNFI = 0.93. The model explained 56% of the variance for psychological CPV at T2 and 26% of the variance for physical CPV at T2.
All autoregressive effects were statistically significant, indicating the stability of CPV over time. Psychological CPV at T1 predicted physical CPV at T2. II at T1 predicted psychological CPV at T2. The other two psychopathic trait dimensions did not predict changes in CPV over time. Neither of the two-way interaction terms between the psychopathic trait dimensions was statistically significant for physical CPV at T2 (GM × CU [0.00 (0.01); p = 0.984], GM × II [0.00 (0.01); p = 0.603], and CU × II [0.01 (0.01); p = 0.211]). However, the interaction of the three psychopathic trait dimensions at T1 (GM × CU × II) predicted physical CPV at T2. Figure 2 represents the three-way interaction. The results indicated that II predicts CPV most when it is combined with high CU and low GM. The examination of the slopes indicated that II was only related to physical CPV when CU was high (Slope 1 [High GM], β = 0.017, t = 2.95, p = 0.003 and Slope 2 [Low GM], β = 0.027, t = 2.82, p = 0.005). II was not related to physical CPV when CU was low (Slope 3 [High GM], β = 0.007, t = 0.76, p = 0.44 and Slope 4 [Low GM], β =  −0.011, t =  −1.41, p = 0.16). Furthermore, the results indicated that the slope difference between (1) and (2) was significant, t =  −1.97, p = 0.049, indicating that, when CU is high, the impact of II on physical CPV is higher when GM is low than when it is high. Further, the slope differences between (1) and (4) and between (2) and (4) were statistically significant (t = 2.27, p = 0.024 and t = 3.73; p < 0.001, respectively), indicating that when CU is high (Slopes 1 and 2); the slope is higher than when both CU and GM are low (Slope 4).

Sex Differences for the Predictive Model

To examine whether the predictive model was sex invariant, we performed a multiple group analysis. For this purpose, the following steps were carried out. First, we estimated the model for girls and boys separately. The fit indices were adequate for girls, χ2(30, N = 463) = 94.86, RMSEA = 0.068, 90% CI [0.053, 0.084], CFI = 0.95, NNFI = 0.92 and for boys, χ2(30, N = 302) = 100.37, RMSEA = 0.088, 90% CI [0.070, 0.011], CFI = 0.96, NNFI = 0.92. Second, the configural invariance of the model was tested to demonstrate that the pattern of the fixed and free parameters was equivalent across the subsamples, χ2 (60, N = 765) = 194.940, RMSEA = 0.0768, 90% CI [0.0649, 0.0889], CFI = 0.956, NNFI = 0.919, SRMR = 0.186. Finally, we estimated a model in which longitudinal paths were constricted to be equal across both subsamples. This constriction did not increase χ2 significantly, ∆χ2(12, N = 765) = 11.145, p = 0.516, which indicates that the overall pattern of paths was similar for girls and boys.

Discussion

This study examined the longitudinal relationships between psychopathic trait dimensions (GM, CU, and II) and psychological and physical CPV in adolescents. Overall, the results showed that psychopathic trait dimensions had a minimal role in CPV. At the cross-sectional level, all three psychopathic trait dimensions were positively related to psychological and physical CPV, although the effect sizes were small. At the longitudinal level, only II predicted psychological CPV six months later. Moreover, the results indicated a second-order interaction between psychopathic trait dimensions, which reveals the importance of the conjoint action of these trait dimensions.
Regarding the role of II as a predictor of psychological CPV, Rico et al. (2017) also found a positive relationship between attentional impulsivity and psychological CPV, but not physical CPV. To interpret these results, an additional explanation is necessary. As a result, Rico et al. (2017) evaluated three dimensions of impulsivity: attentional impulsivity with a predominant component of cognitive or emotional instability; motor impulsivity in which motor activation prevails; and lack of planning with lack of self-control (Barratt et al., 1997). Thus, attentional impulsivity was related to psychological CPV, motor impulsivity was related to psychological and physical CPV, and lack of planning was not related to either type of CPV. The II evaluated in this study as a dimension of psychopathy has an important component of emotional instability (Andershed et al., 2002). Therefore, it seems that impulsiveness, which is understood as emotional instability or a lack of reflecting before acting, is imperative in predicting psychological aggression, such as insulting or making threats against one’s parents, while for physical CPV, additional motor activation becomes more important.
Although simple trait dimension interactions were not related to either type of CPV, the three-way interaction of psychopathic trait dimensions predicted physical CPV. Thus, II is more strongly associated with physical CPV when concurrent with high levels of UC and low levels of GM. Furthermore, within this three-way interaction, the moderating role of CU is especially relevant, as when CU is low, II is not significantly associated with physical CPV. Overall, these results expand current knowledge on CU’s role in the development of physical CPV. Thus, it seems that for physical CPV, alongside the important roles of emotional instability or lack of reflection, lack of empathy or affective indifference in adolescents also plays a substantial role. This follows the findings of Ibabe and Jaureguizar (2010), who indicated that young offenders who committed CPV also lacked empathy. A possible explanation for this may be that adolescents in community samples who perpetrate physical CPV share more characteristics with adolescents in offender samples, in whom physical CPV levels tend to be much higher (Kennedy et al., 2010; Purcell et al., 2014).
The present results also indicated stability in CPV behaviors over time, consistent with previous studies (Calvete et al., 2015a). Furthermore, the perpetration of psychological CPV predicted physical CPV, supporting the idea that CPV has an escalation component (Pereira & Bertino, 2009), with a tendency to worsen over time (Del Hoyo-Bilbao et al., 2018a). These data highlight the relevance of implementing preventive interventions.
Finally, regarding the sex differences in psychopathic trait dimensions, boys scored higher in the CU and GM trait dimensions. These results are consistent with previous studies (Andershed et al., 2018; Fanti et al., 2009). In the case of II, girls obtained higher scores, as has been highlighted in some studies (Orue et al., 2016). However, it must be noted that all the differences were small. Regarding CPV, girls scored higher in psychological CPV, and the effect size was moderate. These results are consistent with studies where girls exercise a greater degree of psychological CPV, especially in community samples (Calvete & Orue, 2016; Lyons et al., 2015; Margolin & Baucom, 2014). In this study, girls also perpetrated more physical CPV. This result is consistent with other studies related to community samples (Calvete et al., 2013, 2015b), although it contrasts studies conducted on judicial samples where there were either no differences (Stickle et al., 2012) or the boys presented more physical aggression (Boxer et al., 2009; Routt & Anderson, 2011). However, the general pattern of associations was similar for boys and girls. These results are consistent with previous studies on the relationships between psychopathic trait dimensions and general aggression (Marsee et al., 2005; Penney & Moretti, 2007).

Limitations and Future Research

In interpreting these results, certain limitations must be considered. First, although self-reports are among the most extensively used measures in studies on CPV and psychopathic traits (Calvete et al., 2012; López-Larrañaga & Orue, 2019), parental measures could also provide a more rounded view of CPV and adolescents’ individual traits (Calvete et al., 2017). Second, the participants belonged to a community sample in which physical CPV levels were very low. This could have directly influenced the relationships studied because it makes it difficult for the possible predictors to explain the outcome variance (Rico et al., 2017). Therefore, we need to study psychopathic trait dimensions in clinical or offender samples of adolescents in which the prevalence of physical CPV is higher (Del Hoyo-Bilbao et al., 2018b). Third, although this study provides valuable information on the individual characteristics of adolescents in the development of CPV, it would be interesting to study the possible moderating role of these variables in the relationships between contextual variables and CPV (e.g., exposure to family violence and CPV). In this way, more information could be provided on the role of personality traits in predicting CPV.

Conclusions

The results of this study indicate that psychopathic trait dimensions in community samples of adolescents play a limited role in CPV development. However, adolescents from offender samples commonly present a wider pattern of antisocial behavior (Kennedy et al., 2010). Thus, as psychopathic trait dimensions have a more relevant role in adolescents who display conduct disorder symptoms (Fanti et al., 2018), future studies should examine whether psychopathic trait dimensions are more relevant for CPV in clinical and offender samples. Finally, this study supports the importance of analyzing the interaction of the three psychopathic trait dimensions to create a better understanding of aggressive behaviors (Lilienfeld, 2018; Somma et al., 2018), including CPV.

Acknowledgements

This study could not have been conducted without the valuable collaboration of the adolescents and the funding of Eusko Jaurlaritza (IT982-16).

Declarations

Informed consent was obtained from legal guardians of the participants. In addition, verbal informed consent was obtained prior to the participation from all adolescents.

Conflict of Interest

Joana Del Hoyo-Bilbao, Izaskun Orue and Esther Calvete declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Levander, S. (2002). Psychopathic traits in non-referred youths: Initial test of a new assessment tool. In E. Blaauw & L. Sheridan (Eds.), Psychopaths: Current international perspectives (pp. 131–158). Elsevier. Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Levander, S. (2002). Psychopathic traits in non-referred youths: Initial test of a new assessment tool. In E. Blaauw & L. Sheridan (Eds.), Psychopaths: Current international perspectives (pp. 131–158). Elsevier.
go back to reference Byrne, B. M. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. LEA. Byrne, B. M. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. LEA.
go back to reference Calvete, E., & Orue, I. (2016). Violencia filio-parental: Prevalencias y razones para las agresiones contra padres y madres. Behavioral Psychology, 24(3), 481–495. Calvete, E., & Orue, I. (2016). Violencia filio-parental: Prevalencias y razones para las agresiones contra padres y madres. Behavioral Psychology, 24(3), 481–495.
go back to reference Calvete, E., Orue, I., & González-Cabrera, J. (2017). Violencia filio parental: Comparando lo que informan los adolescentes y sus progenitores. Revista De Psicología Clínica Con Niños y Adolescentes, 4(1), 9–15. Calvete, E., Orue, I., & González-Cabrera, J. (2017). Violencia filio parental: Comparando lo que informan los adolescentes y sus progenitores. Revista De Psicología Clínica Con Niños y Adolescentes, 4(1), 9–15.
go back to reference Fanti, K. A., Kyranides, M. N., Lordos, A., Colins, O. F., & Andershed, H. (2018). Unique and interactive associations of callous-unemotional traits, impulsivity and grandiosity with child and adolescent conduct disorder symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40, 40–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-018-9655-9CrossRef Fanti, K. A., Kyranides, M. N., Lordos, A., Colins, O. F., & Andershed, H. (2018). Unique and interactive associations of callous-unemotional traits, impulsivity and grandiosity with child and adolescent conduct disorder symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40, 40–49. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10862-018-9655-9CrossRef
go back to reference Gámez-Guadix, M., & Calvete, E. (2012). Violencia filio-parental y su asociación con la exposición a la violencia marital y la agresión de padres a hijos. Psicothema, 24, 277-283. Gámez-Guadix, M., & Calvete, E. (2012). Violencia filio-parental y su asociación con la exposición a la violencia marital y la agresión de padres a hijos. Psicothema, 24, 277-283.
go back to reference Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL 8.80 for Windows. [Computer Software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL 8.80 for Windows. [Computer Software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
go back to reference Orue, I., & Andershed, H. (2015). The youth psychopathic traits inventory-short version in Spanish adolescents—factor structure, reliability, and relation with aggression, bullying, and cyber bullying. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37, 563–575. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-015-9489-7CrossRef Orue, I., & Andershed, H. (2015). The youth psychopathic traits inventory-short version in Spanish adolescents—factor structure, reliability, and relation with aggression, bullying, and cyber bullying. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37, 563–575. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10862-015-9489-7CrossRef
go back to reference Pereira, R., & Bertino, L. (2009). Una comprensión ecológica de la violencia filio-parental. In R. Giraldo & M. I. Gónzalez (Eds.), Violencia familiar (pp. 226–252). Editorial Universidad del Rosario. Pereira, R., & Bertino, L. (2009). Una comprensión ecológica de la violencia filio-parental. In R. Giraldo & M. I. Gónzalez (Eds.), Violencia familiar (pp. 226–252). Editorial Universidad del Rosario.
go back to reference Pereira, R., Loinaz, I., del Hoyo-Bilbao, J., Arrospide, J., Bertino, L., Calvo, A., Montes, Y., & Gutiérrez, M. M. (2017). Propuesta de definición de violencia filio-parental: Consenso de la sociedad española para el estudio de la violencia filio-parental (SEVIFIP) [Proposal for a definition of filioparental violence: Consensus of the Spanish society for the study of filio-parental violence (SEVIFIP)]. Papeles del Psicólogo, 38, 216–233. https://doi.org/10.23923/pap.psicol2017.2839 Pereira, R., Loinaz, I., del Hoyo-Bilbao, J., Arrospide, J., Bertino, L., Calvo, A., Montes, Y., & Gutiérrez, M. M. (2017). Propuesta de definición de violencia filio-parental: Consenso de la sociedad española para el estudio de la violencia filio-parental (SEVIFIP) [Proposal for a definition of filioparental violence: Consensus of the Spanish society for the study of filio-parental violence (SEVIFIP)]. Papeles del Psicólogo, 38, 216–233. https://​doi.​org/​10.​23923/​pap.​psicol2017.​2839
go back to reference Preacher, K. J., & Coffman, D. L. (2006). Computing power and minimum sample size for RMSEA [computer software]. Available from http://quantpsy.org/. Accessed 15 June 2021. Preacher, K. J., & Coffman, D. L. (2006). Computing power and minimum sample size for RMSEA [computer software]. Available from http://​quantpsy.​org/​. Accessed 15 June 2021.
go back to reference Somma, A., Andershed, H., Borroni, S., Salekin, R. T., & Fossati, A. (2018). Psychopathic personality traits in relation to self-report delinquency in adolescence: Should we mind about interaction effects? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40(1), 69–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-018-9658-6CrossRef Somma, A., Andershed, H., Borroni, S., Salekin, R. T., & Fossati, A. (2018). Psychopathic personality traits in relation to self-report delinquency in adolescence: Should we mind about interaction effects? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40(1), 69–78. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10862-018-9658-6CrossRef
go back to reference Van Baardewijk, Y., Andershed, H., Stegge, H., Nilsson, K. W., Scholte, E., & Vermeiren, R. (2010). Development and tests of short versions of the youth psychopathic traits inventory and the youth psychopathic traits inventory-child version. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 26(2), 122–128. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a00001CrossRef Van Baardewijk, Y., Andershed, H., Stegge, H., Nilsson, K. W., Scholte, E., & Vermeiren, R. (2010). Development and tests of short versions of the youth psychopathic traits inventory and the youth psychopathic traits inventory-child version. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 26(2), 122–128. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1027/​1015-5759/​a00001CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Interaction of Psychopathic Traits Dimensions in the Prediction of Psychological and Physical Child-to-Parent Violence in Adolescents
Auteurs
Joana Del Hoyo-Bilbao
Izaskun Orue
Esther Calvete
Publicatiedatum
04-11-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment / Uitgave 1/2022
Print ISSN: 0882-2689
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3505
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-021-09940-w

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2022

Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 1/2022 Naar de uitgave