Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2007

01-01-2007 | Original Article

Instruction-induced feature binding

Auteurs: Dorit Wenke, Robert Gaschler, Dieter Nattkemper

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In order to test whether or not instructions specifying the stimulus–response (S–R) mappings for a new task suffice to create bindings between specified stimulus and response features, we developed a dual task paradigm of the ABBA type in which participants saw new S–R instructions for the A-task in the beginning of each trial. Immediately after the A-task instructions, participants had to perform a logically independent B-task. The imperative stimulus for the A-task was presented after the B-task had been executed. The present data show that the instructed S–R mappings influence performance on the embedded B-task, even when they (1) have never been practiced, and (2) are irrelevant with respect to the B-task. These results imply that instructions can induce bindings between S- and R-features without prior execution of the task at hand.
Voetnoten
1
The original idea for this paradigm was shaped during informal talks with Nachshon Meiran during the ESCOP conference in Edinburgh, 2001. We would like to thank Nachshon for his suggestions and his advice regarding this research project.
 
2
There was no hint of an effect in the inter-response times of those size-task responses that required two consecutive keypresses, implying that research participants planned (and finished) selection of their double-press responses before pressing the space bar for the first time.
 
3
We chose the difference between compatible and neutral trials instead of the overall difference between target-overlapping and non-overlapping trials as an index of overlap costs (i.e., the costs of target-identity repetition compared to non-overlap of target letters across tasks), because it appeared to be the more conservative measure that is less confounded by the extremely slow reactions on spatially incompatible overlapping trials (i.e., by the compatibility effect).
 
4
Note that the resulting task representations are unlikely to be of verbal nature. First, it is hard to see how verbal coding would affect size task performance when the instructed S–R mappings are not even needed in the size task. Second, as mentioned in the Discussion part of Exp. 1, we carried out a replication experiment of Exp. 1 in which participants were required to hold a tongue depressor in their mouths that supposedly blocked their articulatory apparatus, thereby impairing (sub-)vocal rehearsal. Thus, even if there was a way for verbal codes to interfere with size task performance, their impact should have been reduced with tongue depressor. However, the tongue depressor results largely mirrored the Exp. 1 results. Hence, one may speculate that quasi-perceptual or conceptual codes were integrated during some sort of cognitive simulation during instruction understanding, that is, when situation models of the instructed situations were constructed (e.g., Barsalou, 1999; Glenberg & Robertson, 2000).
 
5
We are currently running an additional experiment that directly tests whether encounter-based binding makes any difference with respect to the tasks used in the present study. This experiment differs from the present Exp. 1 in that, similar to Stoet and Hommel (1999), the imperative target letter for the identity task is presented before the size-task, hence allowing response selection (but not execution) in advance of performing the embedded task.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ach, N. (1910). Ü ber den Willensakt und das Temperament. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung. Leipzig: Verlag von Quelle und Meyer. Ach, N. (1910). Ü ber den Willensakt und das Temperament. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung. Leipzig: Verlag von Quelle und Meyer.
go back to reference Allport, A. D., Tipper, S. P., & Chmiel, N. R. J. (1985). Perceptual integration and postcategorical filtering. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 107–132). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Allport, A. D., Tipper, S. P., & Chmiel, N. R. J. (1985). Perceptual integration and postcategorical filtering. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 107–132). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2005). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions (this issue). Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2005). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions (this issue).
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory & Language, 43, 379–401. Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory & Language, 43, 379–401.
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRef Gollwitzer, P. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRef
go back to reference Gordon, R. D., & Irwin, D. E. (1996). What’s in an object file? Evidence from priming studies. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 1260–1277. Gordon, R. D., & Irwin, D. E. (1996). What’s in an object file? Evidence from priming studies. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 1260–1277.
go back to reference Henderson, J. M. (1994). Two representational systems in dynamic visual identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 410–426.CrossRef Henderson, J. M. (1994). Two representational systems in dynamic visual identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 410–426.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 247–273). Cambridge: MIT Press. Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 247–273). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2005). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice (this issue). Hommel, B. (2005). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice (this issue).
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11, 483–521.CrossRef Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11, 483–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 175–219.CrossRefPubMed Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 175–219.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2005). Task switching: On the origin of response congruency effects. Psychological Research (in press). Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2005). Task switching: On the origin of response congruency effects. Psychological Research (in press).
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Gajewski, P. D. (2004). Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 302–306. Kleinsorge, T., & Gajewski, P. D. (2004). Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 302–306.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Gajewski, P. D. (2005). Pending intentions: Effects of prospective task encoding on the performance of another task. Psychological Research (in press). Kleinsorge, T., & Gajewski, P. D. (2005). Pending intentions: Effects of prospective task encoding on the performance of another task. Psychological Research (in press).
go back to reference Koch, I. & Prinz, W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 192–201.CrossRef Koch, I. & Prinz, W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 192–201.CrossRef
go back to reference Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.CrossRefPubMed Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 575–599.CrossRefPubMed Logan, G. D., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 575–599.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Bryck, R. L. (2005). Sticky rules: Integration between abstract rules and specific actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 337–350.PubMedCrossRef Mayr, U., & Bryck, R. L. (2005). Sticky rules: Integration between abstract rules and specific actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 337–350.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 377–399). Cambridge: MIT Press. Meiran, N. (2000). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 377–399). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.CrossRefPubMed Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oriet, C., Stevanovski, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Feature binding and episodic retrieval in blindness for congruent stimuli: Evidence from analyses of sequential congruency (this issue). Oriet, C., Stevanovski, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Feature binding and episodic retrieval in blindness for congruent stimuli: Evidence from analyses of sequential congruency (this issue).
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.PubMedCrossRef Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.CrossRefPubMed Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.CrossRef Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.CrossRef
go back to reference Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 538–552). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 538–552). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Verbruggen, F., Liefooghe, B., Szmalec, A., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005). Inhibiting responses when switching: Does it matter? Experimental Psychology, 52 (in press). Verbruggen, F., Liefooghe, B., Szmalec, A., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005). Inhibiting responses when switching: Does it matter? Experimental Psychology, 52 (in press).
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMed Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2005). Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task-switching: Negative priming and competitor priming. Memory & Cognition (in press). Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2005). Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task-switching: Negative priming and competitor priming. Memory & Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Wenke, D., & Frensch, P.A. (2005). The influence of task instructions on action coding: Constraint Setting or Direct Coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 803–819.CrossRefPubMed Wenke, D., & Frensch, P.A. (2005). The influence of task instructions on action coding: Constraint Setting or Direct Coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 803–819.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wühr, P., & Müsseler, J. (2001). Time course of the blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1260–1270.CrossRefPubMed Wühr, P., & Müsseler, J. (2001). Time course of the blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1260–1270.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Instruction-induced feature binding
Auteurs
Dorit Wenke
Robert Gaschler
Dieter Nattkemper
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0038-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2007

Psychological Research 1/2007 Naar de uitgave