Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2008

01-01-2008 | Original Article

Input monitoring and response selection as components of executive control in pro-saccades and anti-saccades

Auteurs: André Vandierendonck, Maud Deschuyteneer, Ann Depoorter, Denis Drieghe

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Several studies have shown that anti-saccades, more than pro-saccades, are executed under executive control. It is argued that executive control subsumes a variety of controlled processes. The present study tested whether some of these underlying processes are involved in the execution of anti-saccades. An experiment is reported in which two such processes were parametrically varied, namely input monitoring and response selection. This resulted in four selective interference conditions obtained by factorially combining the degree of input monitoring and the presence of response selection in the interference task. The four tasks were combined with a primary task which required the participants to perform either pro-saccades or anti-saccades. By comparison of performance in these dual-task conditions and performance in single-task conditions, it was shown that anti-saccades, but not pro-saccades, were delayed when the secondary task required input monitoring or response selection. The results are discussed with respect to theoretical attempts to deconstruct the concept of executive control.
Voetnoten
1
This could be taken as an indication that the effects of input monitoring and response selection are not additive. Peculiar about this interaction is that the combination of fixed presentation with the simple RT task tends to be slower than the combination of the random presentation with simple RT. The former combination is more vulnerable to the occurrence of anticipatory responses. It seems quite likely that after an anticipation, the next response is slowed resulting in a slower response on average.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allain, S., Carbonnell, L., Burle, B., Hasbroucq, T., & Vidal, F. (2004). On-line executive control: An electromyographic study. Psychophysiology, 41, 113–116.PubMedCrossRef Allain, S., Carbonnell, L., Burle, B., Hasbroucq, T., & Vidal, F. (2004). On-line executive control: An electromyographic study. Psychophysiology, 41, 113–116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28.CrossRef Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp.47–89). NewYork: Academic. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp.47–89). NewYork: Academic.
go back to reference Briand, K. A., Strallow, D., Hening, W., Poizner, H., & Sereno, A. B. (1999). Control of voluntary and reflexive saccades in Parkinson’s disease. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 38–48.CrossRef Briand, K. A., Strallow, D., Hening, W., Poizner, H., & Sereno, A. B. (1999). Control of voluntary and reflexive saccades in Parkinson’s disease. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 38–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Burgess, P. W. (1997). Theory and methodology in executive function research. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.) Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 81–116). Hove: Psychology Press. Burgess, P. W. (1997). Theory and methodology in executive function research. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.) Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 81–116). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Carbonnell, L., Hasbroucq, T., Grapperon, J., & Vidal, F. (2004). Response selection and motor areas: A behavioural and electrophysiological study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 2164–2174.PubMedCrossRef Carbonnell, L., Hasbroucq, T., Grapperon, J., & Vidal, F. (2004). Response selection and motor areas: A behavioural and electrophysiological study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 2164–2174.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deschuyteneer, M., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005a). Are ‘input monitoring’ and ‘response selection’ involved in solving simple mental additions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 343–370.CrossRef Deschuyteneer, M., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005a). Are ‘input monitoring’ and ‘response selection’ involved in solving simple mental additions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 343–370.CrossRef
go back to reference Deschuyteneer, M., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005b). The role of “response selection” and “input monitoring” in solving simple arithmetical products. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1472–1483. Deschuyteneer, M., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005b). The role of “response selection” and “input monitoring” in solving simple arithmetical products. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1472–1483.
go back to reference Deschuyteneer, M., Vandierendonck, A., & Coeman, P. (2005). Exploring the role of different executive processes in solving simple mental subtractions and divisions (submitted for publication). Deschuyteneer, M., Vandierendonck, A., & Coeman, P. (2005). Exploring the role of different executive processes in solving simple mental subtractions and divisions (submitted for publication).
go back to reference Deschuyteneer, M., Vandierendonck, A., & Muyllaert, I. (2006). Does solution of mental arithmetic problems such as 2 + 6 and 3 × 8 rely on the process of ‘memory updating’? Experimental Psychology (in press). Deschuyteneer, M., Vandierendonck, A., & Muyllaert, I. (2006). Does solution of mental arithmetic problems such as 2 + 6 and 3 × 8 rely on the process of ‘memory updating’? Experimental Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Eenshuistra, R. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Age-related changes in anti-saccade task performance: Inhibitory control or working-memory engagement? Brain and Cognition, 56, 177–188.PubMed Eenshuistra, R. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Age-related changes in anti-saccade task performance: Inhibitory control or working-memory engagement? Brain and Cognition, 56, 177–188.PubMed
go back to reference Everling, S., & Fischer, B. (1998). The anti-saccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies. Neuropsychologia, 36, 885–899.PubMedCrossRef Everling, S., & Fischer, B. (1998). The anti-saccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies. Neuropsychologia, 36, 885–899.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Feifel, D., Farber, R. H., Clementz, B. A., Perry, W., & Anllo-Vento, L. (2004). Inhibitory deficits in ocular motor behavior in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 333–339.PubMedCrossRef Feifel, D., Farber, R. H., Clementz, B. A., Perry, W., & Anllo-Vento, L. (2004). Inhibitory deficits in ocular motor behavior in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 333–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 661–721.PubMedCrossRef Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 661–721.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frith, C. D., & Done, D. J. (1986). Routes to action in reaction-time tasks. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 48, 169–177.CrossRef Frith, C. D., & Done, D. J. (1986). Routes to action in reaction-time tasks. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 48, 169–177.CrossRef
go back to reference Fukushima, J., Fukushima, K., Miyasaka, K., & Yamashita, I. (1994). Voluntary control of saccadic eye movement in patients with frontal cortical lesions and Parkinsonian patients with thatin schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry, 36, 21–30.PubMedCrossRef Fukushima, J., Fukushima, K., Miyasaka, K., & Yamashita, I. (1994). Voluntary control of saccadic eye movement in patients with frontal cortical lesions and Parkinsonian patients with thatin schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry, 36, 21–30.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A., & Douglas, R. M. (1985). Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 58, 455–472.CrossRef Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A., & Douglas, R. M. (1985). Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 58, 455–472.CrossRef
go back to reference Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research, 18, 1279–1296.PubMedCrossRef Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research, 18, 1279–1296.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hallett, P. E., & Adams, B. D. (1980). The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task. Vision Research, 20, 329–339.PubMedCrossRef Hallett, P. E., & Adams, B. D. (1980). The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task. Vision Research, 20, 329–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2005). Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: A single-case study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 1–13. Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2005). Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: A single-case study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 1–13.
go back to reference Henik, A., Rafal, R., & Rhodes, D. (1994). Endogenously generated and visually guided saccades after lesion of the human frontal eye fields. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 400–411.CrossRef Henik, A., Rafal, R., & Rhodes, D. (1994). Endogenously generated and visually guided saccades after lesion of the human frontal eye fields. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 400–411.CrossRef
go back to reference Hutton, S. B., Huddy, V., Barnes, T. R. E., Robbins, T. W., Crawford, T. J., Kennard, C., & Joyce, E. M. (2004). The relationship between anti-saccades, smooth pursuit, and executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 553–559.PubMedCrossRef Hutton, S. B., Huddy, V., Barnes, T. R. E., Robbins, T. W., Crawford, T. J., Kennard, C., & Joyce, E. M. (2004). The relationship between anti-saccades, smooth pursuit, and executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 553–559.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183.CrossRef Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Klein, C. (2001). Developmental functions for saccadic eye movement parameters derived from pro-and anti-saccade tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 139, 1–17. Klein, C. (2001). Developmental functions for saccadic eye movement parameters derived from pro-and anti-saccade tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 139, 1–17.
go back to reference Klein, C., Heinks, T., Andresen, B., Berg, P., & Moritz, S. (2000). Impaired modulation of the saccadic contingent negative variation preceding anti-saccades in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 978–990.PubMedCrossRef Klein, C., Heinks, T., Andresen, B., Berg, P., & Moritz, S. (2000). Impaired modulation of the saccadic contingent negative variation preceding anti-saccades in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 978–990.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koval, M. J., Ford, K. A., & Everling, S. (2004). Effect of stimulus probability on anti-saccade error rates. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 268–272.CrossRef Koval, M. J., Ford, K. A., & Everling, S. (2004). Effect of stimulus probability on anti-saccade error rates. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 268–272.CrossRef
go back to reference Kristíansson, A., Chen, Y., & Nakayama, K. (2001). Less attention is more in the preparation of anti-saccades, but not pro-saccades. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 1037–1042.CrossRef Kristíansson, A., Chen, Y., & Nakayama, K. (2001). Less attention is more in the preparation of anti-saccades, but not pro-saccades. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 1037–1042.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). Don’t look! Don’t touch! Inhibitory control of eye and hand movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 107–112. Logan, G. D., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). Don’t look! Don’t touch! Inhibitory control of eye and hand movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 107–112.
go back to reference Massen, C. (2004). Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the anti-saccade task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 475–498. Massen, C. (2004). Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the anti-saccade task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 475–498.
go back to reference Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 95–103.PubMedCrossRef Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 95–103.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedCrossRef Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mort, D. J., Perry, R. J., Mannan, S. K., Hodgson, T. L., Anderson, E., Quest, R., McRobbie, D., McBride, A., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2003). Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. NeuroImage, 18, 231–246.PubMedCrossRef Mort, D. J., Perry, R. J., Mannan, S. K., Hodgson, T. L., Anderson, E., Quest, R., McRobbie, D., McBride, A., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2003). Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. NeuroImage, 18, 231–246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mostofsky, S. H., Lasker, A. G., Singer, H. S., Denckla, M. B., & Zee, D. S. (2001). Oculomotor abnormalities in boys with Tourette syndrome with and without ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 1464–1472.CrossRef Mostofsky, S. H., Lasker, A. G., Singer, H. S., Denckla, M. B., & Zee, D. S. (2001). Oculomotor abnormalities in boys with Tourette syndrome with and without ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 1464–1472.CrossRef
go back to reference Nieuwenhuis, S., Broerse, A., Nielen, M. M. A., & de Jong, R. (2004). A goal activation approach to the study of executive function: An application to anti-saccade tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56, 198–214.PubMedCrossRef Nieuwenhuis, S., Broerse, A., Nielen, M. M. A., & de Jong, R. (2004). A goal activation approach to the study of executive function: An application to anti-saccade tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56, 198–214.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference O’Driscoll, G. A., Depatie, L., Holahan, A. L. V., Savion-Lemieux, T., Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., & Douglas, V. I. (2005). Executive functions and methylphenidate response in subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1452–1460.PubMedCrossRef O’Driscoll, G. A., Depatie, L., Holahan, A. L. V., Savion-Lemieux, T., Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., & Douglas, V. I. (2005). Executive functions and methylphenidate response in subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1452–1460.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olincy, A., Ross, R. G., Youngd, D. A., & Freedman, R. (1997). Age diminishes performance on an anti-saccade eye movement task. Neurobiology of Aging, 18, 483–489.PubMedCrossRef Olincy, A., Ross, R. G., Youngd, D. A., & Freedman, R. (1997). Age diminishes performance on an anti-saccade eye movement task. Neurobiology of Aging, 18, 483–489.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olk, B., Chang, E., Kingstone, A., & Ro, T. (2006). Modulation of anti-saccades by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 76–82.PubMedCrossRef Olk, B., Chang, E., Kingstone, A., & Ro, T. (2006). Modulation of anti-saccades by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 76–82.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pearson, D. G., & Sahraie, A. (2003). Oculomotor control and the maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1089–1111. Pearson, D. G., & Sahraie, A. (2003). Oculomotor control and the maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1089–1111.
go back to reference Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2005). Switching of response modalities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 1325–1338. Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2005). Switching of response modalities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 1325–1338.
go back to reference Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., & Agid, Y. (1991). Cortical control of reflexive visually guided saccades. Brain, 114, 1473–1485.PubMedCrossRef Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., & Agid, Y. (1991). Cortical control of reflexive visually guided saccades. Brain, 114, 1473–1485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Muri, R. M., Nyffeler, T., & Milea, D. (2005). The role of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 239–251.PubMedCrossRef Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Muri, R. M., Nyffeler, T., & Milea, D. (2005). The role of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 239–251.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ploner, C. J., Gaymard, B. M., Rivaud-Péchoux, S., & Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (2005). The prefrontal substrate of reflexive saccade inhibition in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1159–1165.PubMedCrossRef Ploner, C. J., Gaymard, B. M., Rivaud-Péchoux, S., & Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (2005). The prefrontal substrate of reflexive saccade inhibition in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1159–1165.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roberts, R. J. Jr., Hager, L. D., & Heron, C. (1994). Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the anti-saccade task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 374–393.CrossRef Roberts, R. J. Jr., Hager, L. D., & Heron, C. (1994). Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the anti-saccade task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 374–393.CrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Schubert, T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 408–425.CrossRef Schubert, T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 408–425.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.PubMedCrossRef Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spector, A., & Biederman, I. (1976). Mental set and mental shift revisited. Journal of Psychology, 89, 669–679.CrossRef Spector, A., & Biederman, I. (1976). Mental set and mental shift revisited. Journal of Psychology, 89, 669–679.CrossRef
go back to reference Stuyven, E., Van der Goten, K., Vandierendonck, A., Claeys, K., & Crevits, L. .(2000). Saccadic eye movements under conditions of cognitive load. Acta Psychologica, 104, 69–85.PubMedCrossRef Stuyven, E., Van der Goten, K., Vandierendonck, A., Claeys, K., & Crevits, L. .(2000). Saccadic eye movements under conditions of cognitive load. Acta Psychologica, 104, 69–85.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Szmalec, A., Vandierendonck, A., & Kemps, E. (2005). Response selection involves executive control: Evidence from the selective interference paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 33, 531–541. Szmalec, A., Vandierendonck, A., & Kemps, E. (2005). Response selection involves executive control: Evidence from the selective interference paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 33, 531–541.
go back to reference Unsworth, N., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Working memory capacity and the anti-saccade task: Individual differences in voluntary saccade control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1302–1321.PubMedCrossRef Unsworth, N., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Working memory capacity and the anti-saccade task: Individual differences in voluntary saccade control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1302–1321.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vandierendonck, A. (2000). Bias and processing capacity in generation of random time intervals. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 1, 205–233. Vandierendonck, A. (2000). Bias and processing capacity in generation of random time intervals. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 1, 205–233.
go back to reference Walker, R., Husain, M., Hodgson, T. L., Harrison, J., & Kennard, C. (1998). Saccadic eye movement and working memory deficits following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1141–1159.PubMedCrossRef Walker, R., Husain, M., Hodgson, T. L., Harrison, J., & Kennard, C. (1998). Saccadic eye movement and working memory deficits following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1141–1159.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Input monitoring and response selection as components of executive control in pro-saccades and anti-saccades
Auteurs
André Vandierendonck
Maud Deschuyteneer
Ann Depoorter
Denis Drieghe
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0078-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2008

Psychological Research 1/2008 Naar de uitgave