Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2011

01-01-2011 | Original Article

Increasing stimulus intensity does not affect sensorimotor synchronization

Auteurs: Anita Białuńska, Simone Dalla Bella, Piotr Jaśkowski

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

When people synchronize taps with isochronously presented stimuli, taps usually precede the pacing stimuli [negative mean asynchrony (NMA)]. One explanation of NMA [sensory accumulation model (SAM), Aschersleben in Brain Cogn 48:66–79, 2002] is that more time is needed to generate a central code for kinesthetic-tactile information than for auditory or visual stimuli. The SAM predicts that raising the intensity of the pacing stimuli shortens the time for their sensory accumulation, thereby increasing NMA. This prediction was tested by asking participants to synchronize finger force pulses with target isochronous stimuli with various intensities. In addition, participants performed a simple reaction-time task, for comparison. Higher intensity led to shorter reaction times. However, intensity manipulation did not affect NMA in the synchronization task. This finding is not consistent with the predictions based on the SAM. Discrepancies in sensitivity to stimulus intensity between sensorimotor synchronization and reaction-time tasks point to the involvement of different timing mechanisms in these two tasks.
Voetnoten
1
Note, however, that self-generated tactile stimuli are perceived as weaker than the same stimuli externally generated (Bays, Wolpert, & Flanagan, 2005; Blakemore, Wolpert, & Frith, 1998). Therefore, differences in transmission speed between self-generated and externally generated tactile feedback, affecting perceived stimulus intensity, may still account for the NMA.
 
2
1.5 Newtons correspond to 300 µV, which is the force intensity typically needed to obtain a response using the computer keyboard or a response pad. One Newton is equivalent to the force needed to give a mass of 1 kg an acceleration of 1 m/s2.
 
3
The Greenhouse-Geisser correction for inhomogeneity of variance was applied whenever appropriate; uncorrected degrees of freedom, epsilon value and probability level following correction are reported.
 
4
Results in both conditions were confirmed by non-parametric tests (i.e. Friedman’s test).
 
5
High latency jitter of low intensity stimuli predicts many theories of stimuli detection (Grice, 1972; Ractliff & Rouder, 1998; Smith, 1995).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aschersleben, G. (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronizations. Brain and Cognition, 48, 66–79.CrossRefPubMed Aschersleben, G. (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronizations. Brain and Cognition, 48, 66–79.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., Gehrke, J., & Prinz, W. (2001). Tapping with peripheral nerve block: A role for tactile feedback in the timing of movements. Experimental Brain Research, 136, 331–339.CrossRef Aschersleben, G., Gehrke, J., & Prinz, W. (2001). Tapping with peripheral nerve block: A role for tactile feedback in the timing of movements. Experimental Brain Research, 136, 331–339.CrossRef
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., Gehrke, J., & Prinz, W. (2004). A psychophysical approach to action timing. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.), Psychophysics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition (pp. 117–136). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Aschersleben, G., Gehrke, J., & Prinz, W. (2004). A psychophysical approach to action timing. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.), Psychophysics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition (pp. 117–136). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1995). Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(3), 305–317. Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1995). Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(3), 305–317.
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., Stenneken, P., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX (pp. 227–244). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Aschersleben, G., Stenneken, P., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX (pp. 227–244). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Bays, P. M., Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2005). Perception of the consequences of self-action is temporally tuned and event-driven. Current Biology, 15, 1125–1128.CrossRefPubMed Bays, P. M., Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2005). Perception of the consequences of self-action is temporally tuned and event-driven. Current Biology, 15, 1125–1128.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Billon, M., Semjen, A., Cole, J., & Gauthier, G. (1996). The role of sensory information in the production of periodic finger-tapping sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 110, 243–251.CrossRef Billon, M., Semjen, A., Cole, J., & Gauthier, G. (1996). The role of sensory information in the production of periodic finger-tapping sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 110, 243–251.CrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 635–640.CrossRefPubMed Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 635–640.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cardoso-Leite, P., Gorea, A., & Mamassian, P. (2007). Temporal-order judgment and simple reaction times: Evidence for a common processing system. Journal of Vision, 7(6), 1–14.CrossRef Cardoso-Leite, P., Gorea, A., & Mamassian, P. (2007). Temporal-order judgment and simple reaction times: Evidence for a common processing system. Journal of Vision, 7(6), 1–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunlap, K. (1910). Reactions on rhythmic stimuli, with attempt to synchronize. Psychological Review, 17, 399–416.CrossRef Dunlap, K. (1910). Reactions on rhythmic stimuli, with attempt to synchronize. Psychological Review, 17, 399–416.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1980). Les synchronisations sensori-motrices aux rythmes (Sensorimotor synchronizations to rhythms). In J. Requin (Ed.), Anticipation et comportement (pp. 233–257). Paris: Centre National. Fraisse, P. (1980). Les synchronisations sensori-motrices aux rythmes (Sensorimotor synchronizations to rhythms). In J. Requin (Ed.), Anticipation et comportement (pp. 233–257). Paris: Centre National.
go back to reference Gehrke, J. (1995). Sensorimotor synchronization: The intensity of afferent feedback affects the timing of movements (paper 15/95). Munich: Max Planck Institut for Psychological Research. Gehrke, J. (1995). Sensorimotor synchronization: The intensity of afferent feedback affects the timing of movements (paper 15/95). Munich: Max Planck Institut for Psychological Research.
go back to reference Goebl, W., & Palmer, C. (2008). Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano performance. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 471–479.CrossRef Goebl, W., & Palmer, C. (2008). Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano performance. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 471–479.CrossRef
go back to reference Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronizationstrategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, 73–77.PubMed Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronizationstrategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, 73–77.PubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P. (1992). Temporal-order judgment and reaction time for short and long stimuli. Psychological Research, 54, 141–145.CrossRefPubMed Jaśkowski, P. (1992). Temporal-order judgment and reaction time for short and long stimuli. Psychological Research, 54, 141–145.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P. (1996). Simple reaction time and perception of temporal order: Dissociations and hypotheses. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 707–730.PubMed Jaśkowski, P. (1996). Simple reaction time and perception of temporal order: Dissociations and hypotheses. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 707–730.PubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P. (1999). Reaction time and temporal-order judgment as measures of perceptual latency: The problem of dissociations. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spacial and temporal events (pp. 265–283). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Jaśkowski, P. (1999). Reaction time and temporal-order judgment as measures of perceptual latency: The problem of dissociations. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spacial and temporal events (pp. 265–283). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., Rybarczyk, K., & Jaroszyk, F. (1994). On the relationship between latency of auditory evoked potentials, simple reaction time and stimulus intensity. Psychological Research, 56, 59–65.CrossRefPubMed Jaśkowski, P., Rybarczyk, K., & Jaroszyk, F. (1994). On the relationship between latency of auditory evoked potentials, simple reaction time and stimulus intensity. Psychological Research, 56, 59–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Wauschkuhn, B., Wascher, E., & Verleger, R. (2000). The influence of time pressure and cue validity on response force in an S1–S2 paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 105, 89–105.CrossRefPubMed Jaśkowski, P., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Wauschkuhn, B., Wascher, E., & Verleger, R. (2000). The influence of time pressure and cue validity on response force in an S1–S2 paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 105, 89–105.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., & Verleger, R. (1993). A clock paradigm to study the relationship between expectancy and response force. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, 163–174.PubMed Jaśkowski, P., & Verleger, R. (1993). A clock paradigm to study the relationship between expectancy and response force. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, 163–174.PubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., & Włodarczyk, D. (2006). Task modulation of the effect of brightness on reaction time and response force. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 98–112.CrossRefPubMed Jaśkowski, P., & Włodarczyk, D. (2006). Task modulation of the effect of brightness on reaction time and response force. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 98–112.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (2000). Principles of neural science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (2000). Principles of neural science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference Kolers, P. A., & Brewster, J. M. (1985). Rhythms and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 11, 150–167.CrossRefPubMed Kolers, P. A., & Brewster, J. M. (1985). Rhythms and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 11, 150–167.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: Theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), The psychology of time. Bingley: Emerald. Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: Theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), The psychology of time. Bingley: Emerald.
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track time varying events. Psychological Review, 106(1), 119–159.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track time varying events. Psychological Review, 106(1), 119–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Lütkenhöner, B., Krumbholz, K., Lammertmann, C., Seither-Preisler, A., Steinsträter, O., & Patterson, R. D. (2003). Localization of primary auditory cortex in humans by magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage, 18, 58–66.CrossRefPubMed Lütkenhöner, B., Krumbholz, K., Lammertmann, C., Seither-Preisler, A., Steinsträter, O., & Patterson, R. D. (2003). Localization of primary auditory cortex in humans by magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage, 18, 58–66.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mates, J. (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence: I. Timing and error corrections. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 463–473.CrossRefPubMed Mates, J. (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence: I. Timing and error corrections. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 463–473.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mates, J. (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence: II Stability analysis, error estimation and simulations. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 475–484.CrossRefPubMed Mates, J. (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence: II Stability analysis, error estimation and simulations. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 475–484.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mates, J., & Aschersleben, G. (2000). Sensorimotor synchronization: The impact of temporally displaced auditory feedback. Acta Psychologica, 104, 29–44.CrossRefPubMed Mates, J., & Aschersleben, G. (2000). Sensorimotor synchronization: The impact of temporally displaced auditory feedback. Acta Psychologica, 104, 29–44.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mates, J., Radil, T., & Pöppel, E. (1992). Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 691–704. Mates, J., Radil, T., & Pöppel, E. (1992). Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 691–704.
go back to reference Menendez, A., Lit, A. (1983). Effects of test-flash and steady background luminance on simple visual reaction time (RT) and perceived simultaneity (PC) (Abstract). Investigative Opthalmology and Visual Science, 24(Suppl), 95. Menendez, A., Lit, A. (1983). Effects of test-flash and steady background luminance on simple visual reaction time (RT) and perceived simultaneity (PC) (Abstract). Investigative Opthalmology and Visual Science, 24(Suppl), 95.
go back to reference Michon, J. A. (1967). Timing in temporal tracking. Assen: van Gorcum. Michon, J. A. (1967). Timing in temporal tracking. Assen: van Gorcum.
go back to reference Miller, J. O., & Schwartz, W. (2006). Dissociations between reaction times and temporal order judgments: A diffusion model approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 2, 394–412.CrossRef Miller, J. O., & Schwartz, W. (2006). Dissociations between reaction times and temporal order judgments: A diffusion model approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 2, 394–412.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J. O., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (1999). Effects of stimulus intensity on the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1454–1471.CrossRef Miller, J. O., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (1999). Effects of stimulus intensity on the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1454–1471.CrossRef
go back to reference Mordkoff, J. T., Miller, J. O., & Roch, A. C. (1996). Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 22, 25–41.CrossRefPubMed Mordkoff, J. T., Miller, J. O., & Roch, A. C. (1996). Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 22, 25–41.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Neumann, O., Esselmann, U., & Klotz, W. (1993). Differential effects of visual-spatial attention on response latency and temporal-order judgment. Psychological Research, 56, 26–34.CrossRefPubMed Neumann, O., Esselmann, U., & Klotz, W. (1993). Differential effects of visual-spatial attention on response latency and temporal-order judgment. Psychological Research, 56, 26–34.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Neumann, O., & Niepel, M. (2004). Timing of perception and perception of time. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.), Psychophys-ics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition (pp. 245–269). Mahwah: Erlbaum. Neumann, O., & Niepel, M. (2004). Timing of perception and perception of time. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.), Psychophys-ics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition (pp. 245–269). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Paillard, J. (1948). Quelques données psychophysiologiques relatives au déclenchement de la commande motrice (Some psychophysiological data relating to the triggering of motor commands). L’Année Psychologique, 47–48, 28–47. Paillard, J. (1948). Quelques données psychophysiologiques relatives au déclenchement de la commande motrice (Some psychophysiological data relating to the triggering of motor commands). L’Année Psychologique, 47–48, 28–47.
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 355–370.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 355–370.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969–992. Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969–992.
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Doggett, R. (2007). Tapping to a very slow beat: A comparison of musicians and non-musicians. Music Perception, 24, 367–376.CrossRef Repp, B. H., & Doggett, R. (2007). Tapping to a very slow beat: A comparison of musicians and non-musicians. Music Perception, 24, 367–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Towards a psychophysics of agency: Detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 469–482.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Towards a psychophysics of agency: Detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 469–482.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68, 252–270.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68, 252–270.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Roufs, J. A. J. (1974). Dynamic properties of vision: V Perception lag and reaction time in relation to flicker and flash thresholds. Vision Research, 14, 853–869.CrossRefPubMed Roufs, J. A. J. (1974). Dynamic properties of vision: V Perception lag and reaction time in relation to flicker and flash thresholds. Vision Research, 14, 853–869.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sanford, A. J. (1971). Effects of changes in the intensity of white noise on simultaneity judgements and simple reaction time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Experimental Psychology, 23(A), 296–303. Sanford, A. J. (1971). Effects of changes in the intensity of white noise on simultaneity judgements and simple reaction time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Experimental Psychology, 23(A), 296–303.
go back to reference Sanford, A. J. (1974). Attention bias and the relation of perception lag to simple reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 443–446.CrossRefPubMed Sanford, A. J. (1974). Attention bias and the relation of perception lag to simple reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 443–446.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spencer, R., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science, 300, 1437–1439.CrossRefPubMed Spencer, R., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science, 300, 1437–1439.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stenneken, P., Aschersleben, G., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Self-induced versus reactive triggering of synchronous movements in a deafferented patient and control subjects. Psychological Research, 66, 40–49.CrossRefPubMed Stenneken, P., Aschersleben, G., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Self-induced versus reactive triggering of synchronous movements in a deafferented patient and control subjects. Psychological Research, 66, 40–49.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thaut, M. H., Miller, R. A., & Schauer, L. M. (1998). Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: Phase vs. period correction. Biological Cybernetics, 79, 241–250.CrossRefPubMed Thaut, M. H., Miller, R. A., & Schauer, L. M. (1998). Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: Phase vs. period correction. Biological Cybernetics, 79, 241–250.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vaughan, J., Mattson, T. R., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (1998). The regulation of contact in rhythmic tapping. In D. A. Rosenbaum & C. E. Collyer (Eds.), Timing of behavior: neural, computational, and psychological perspectives. Cambridge: MIT Press. Vaughan, J., Mattson, T. R., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (1998). The regulation of contact in rhythmic tapping. In D. A. Rosenbaum & C. E. Collyer (Eds.), Timing of behavior: neural, computational, and psychological perspectives. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action 2 (pp. 181–262). London: Academic Press. Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action 2 (pp. 181–262). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Vos, P. G., Mates, J., & van Kruysbergen, N. W. (1995). The perceptual centre of a stimulus as the cue for synchronization to a metronome: Evidence from asynchronies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A(4), 1024–1040. Vos, P. G., Mates, J., & van Kruysbergen, N. W. (1995). The perceptual centre of a stimulus as the cue for synchronization to a metronome: Evidence from asynchronies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A(4), 1024–1040.
go back to reference Waszak, F., & Gorea, A. (2004). A new look on the relation between perceptual and motor responses. Visual Cognition, 11, 947–963.CrossRef Waszak, F., & Gorea, A. (2004). A new look on the relation between perceptual and motor responses. Visual Cognition, 11, 947–963.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Increasing stimulus intensity does not affect sensorimotor synchronization
Auteurs
Anita Białuńska
Simone Dalla Bella
Piotr Jaśkowski
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0282-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2011

Psychological Research 1/2011 Naar de uitgave

Editorial

Editorial