Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research 4/2017

Open Access 15-10-2016

Impact of neurocognitive deficits on patient–proxy agreement regarding health-related quality of life in low-grade glioma patients

Auteurs: Divine E. Ediebah, Jaap C. Reijneveld, Martin J. B. Taphoorn, Corneel Coens, Efstathios Zikos, Neil K. Aaronson, Jan J. Heimans, Andrew Bottomley, Martin Klein, on behalf of the EORTC Quality of Life Department and Patient Reported Outcome and Behavioral Evidence (PROBE)

Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research | Uitgave 4/2017

Abstract

Purpose

Clinical trials in glioma patients with neurocognitive deficits face challenges due to lacking or unreliable patient self-reports on their health-related quality of life (HRQOL). Patient–proxy data could help solve this issue. We determined whether patient–proxy concordance levels were affected by patients’ neurocognitive functioning.

Methods

Patient and patient-by-proxy HRQOL ratings were assessed via SF-36 and EORTC QLQ-BN20, respectively, in 246 patients. Data on neurocognitive functioning were collected on a subgroup of 195 patients. Patient–proxy agreement was measured using the Bland–Altman limit of agreement, the mean difference, the concordance correlation coefficient (CCC), and the percentage difference (PD, ±0, 5, or 10 points). We defined patients to be cognitively impaired (n = 66) or cognitively intact (n = 129) based on their neurocognitive performance.

Results

Patients rated their physical function and general health to be better than their proxies did, while at the same time, patients reported more visual disorders, communication deficits, itchy skin, and problems with bladder control. The cognitively impaired subgroup reported poorer physical functioning, more visual disorders, headaches, itchy skin, and issues with bladder control. In the cognitively intact group, no statistical significant differences were observed between patients and proxies. Not surprisingly, Bland–Altman plots revealed a high agreement between the patient and patient-by-proxy rating in all HRQOL domains ranging from 95 to 99 %. The CCC was fairly high in all HRQOL domains (0.37–0.80), and the percentage of perfect agreement (PD ± 0) ranged from 8.5 to 76.8 %. In the cognitively impaired patients, the mean difference between patients and proxies was overall larger, and accordingly, agreement based on Bland–Altman plots was lower.

Conclusions

The level of agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings of low-grade glioma patients’ HRQOL is generally high. However, patient–proxy agreement is lower in patients with neurocognitive deficits than in patients without neurocognitive deficits.
Opmerkingen

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1007/​s11136-016-1426-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

Health-related quality of life (HRQOL) has become an important secondary outcome measure in clinical trials of glioma patients [1, 2] with all European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer (EORTC) brain tumor clinical trials and most trials by other cancer groups now incorporating HRQOL. HRQOL is an important complement to conventional outcome parameters such as time to tumor progression, overall and progression-free survival, and radiological response might be inadequate or less relevant for meaningful evaluation of this type of treatment [3]. HRQOL in glioma patients is influenced by both tumor- and treatment-related factors [4, 5] with seizures and antiepileptic drugs [6], fatigue [7], anxiety and depression [8], and neurocognitive deficits [9, 10] affecting HRQOL in particular. Apart from negatively affecting HRQOL, neurocognitive deficits may also hamper adequate patient self-reports, as patients’ neurocognitive deficits may render HRQOL patient-reported outcomes through questionnaires unreliable [11]. Exclusion of these patients at the lower end of the neurocognitive spectrum from analyses obviously introduces undesirable bias in the evaluation of patients’ HRQOL during experimental treatments. Moreover, cognitively impaired patients may be less compliant regarding questionnaire-based HRQOL monitoring, thereby introducing another source of bias.
The incorporation of HRQOL estimates of the partner or another close relative or friend (denominated as ‘proxy’) might solve this problem to a large extent. Previous reports indicate that high-grade glioma patient- and proxy-reported HRQOL have a high level of concordance as long as the patient shows no signs of decline in neurocognitive function [12, 13], but differences, particularly in mood-related issues, increase when neurocognitive functioning decreases [13, 14]. While HRQOL is by definition subjective, and it is typically measured with self-reports, it has been suggested that substituting proxy ratings when a patient’s self-report is absent or unreliable should be considered [15]. When differences between patient- and proxy-reported HRQOL ratings develop in the course of the disease (presumably at the time, decline in neurocognitive function becomes an issue), proxy-reported instead of patient-reported HRQOL ratings might be regarded as the most reliable source of information on patients’ HRQOL.
Previous studies reported that low observed correlations between patient- and patient-by-proxy-reported outcomes might be explained by methodological weaknesses such as small sample size, suboptimal reliability, and score variability [13, 14]. This was supported by Bland and Altman, who stated that a single measure such as a correlation coefficient may not be sufficient to summarize agreement adequately [16, 17]. In the present study, by using a wide range of statistical measures of agreement, we investigated patient–proxy HRQOL agreement in a large sample of low-grade glioma (LGG) patients, both with intact and with impaired neurocognitive functioning. The cohort used in this analysis is unique because of the extensive neurocognitive test battery incorporated.
The aim of the present study is to investigate the agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings of HRQOL and to investigate whether the level of neurocognitive functioning influences the level of patient–proxy concordance. We hypothesized that (1) concordance levels are relatively high on mental and physical functioning in cognitively intact patients, and (2) there is a decrease in mental functioning in cognitively impaired patients, with proxies being more negative on patients’ HRQOL.

Patients and methods

For this cross-sectional study, we recruited low-grade glioma (LGG) patients who were disease-free for at least one year following diagnosis and primary treatment, and their proxies. Patients were recruited from neurosurgical centers throughout the Netherlands between February, 1997 and January, 2000. Eligibility was checked with the general practitioner and by a case-note review. Low-grade glioma was classified histologically as astrocytoma, oligodendroglioma, or oligoastrocytoma. Patients were excluded if they used corticosteroids (because use of corticosteroids might indicate non-stable disease), did not have a basic proficiency in the Dutch language, or were unable to communicate adequately. All patients provided written informed consent to be involved on the study, and ethics approvals of the study protocol were obtained from the medical ethics committees of the institutions. The details of the study conduct and clinical outcome have been reported elsewhere [18].
HRQOL was assessed using the MOS SF-36 Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) [19, 20] in conjunction with the European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer Brain Cancer module (QLQ-BN20) [21] to assess additional health problems associated specifically with glioma and its treatment. The MOS SF-36 is a self-report questionnaire developed in the USA as a part of a large, national study of the effect of various forms of health care delivery on patients’ health status and quality of life [22]. It is composed of 36 items, organized into eight multi-item scales assessing: (1) physical functioning; (2) bodily pain; (3) role limitations due to physical health problems; (4) role limitations due to personal or emotional problems; (5) emotional well-being; (6) social functioning; (7) energy/fatigue; and (8) general health perceptions. Summary component scores for physical health (PCS) and mental health (MCS) were also calculated. Higher scores indicate better health. The questionnaire has excellent reliability and validity when employed with diverse patient populations [23, 24]. The SF-36 also has exhibited good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.84) when employed among Dutch cancer patients [25].
The QLQ-BN20, where higher scores indicate more symptoms, is composed of 20 items, organized into five subscales assessing future uncertainty, visual disorder, motor dysfunction, communication deficit, and emotional distress. The remaining seven items assess other disease symptoms, and side effects of treatment found to be prevalent among patients with brain tumors, including headaches, seizures, drowsiness, hair loss, itching, weakness in the legs, and lack of bladder control. The QLQ-BN20 has robust psychometric properties that result from rigorous testing and the development of their use in several international clinical cancer trials [26]. The SF-36 and QLQ-BN20 were completed by (1) the patient; (2) the partner, providing a ‘proxy’ rating of the patients’ HRQOL, i.e., patient-by-proxy. Based on the objective outlined in the introduction, we will limit our analysis of agreement to SF-36 and QLQ-BN20 questionnaires completed by the patient and the partner as a proxy.

Neurocognitive assessments

Neurocognitive functioning refers to an individual’s ability to perceive, store, retrieve, and use sensory and perceptual information from the environment and past experience, and to such mental activities as plans and strategies [18]. A disability score was calculated for every patient; neurocognitive test scores were converted to z-scores, with the mean scores of the healthy controls as a reference. The lower 5th percentile of the healthy controls was used as a cut-off score for cognitive disability [27]. To calculate an overall disability score for every patient, we counted the number of tests on which the patient scored below this cut-off. Application of this algorithm to our data showed that a glioma patient was judged to have a neurocognitive disability if he or she had deviant scores for at least 4 of the 20 tests. Only tests for which healthy controls could be individually matched with LGG patients for age, sex, and educational level were used for this analysis. Unlike, for instance, research concerning patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), there is no consensus on what represents a ‘true’ drop in neurocognitive functioning in brain tumor patients. By applying this strict, clinically based cut-off, we assume that patients with neurocognitive disability will experience limitations in their daily life functioning. Detailed information about the standard tests used to assess cognitive status is shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Neuropsychological tests and corresponding cognitive domains
Intelligence
Dutch adult reading test [30]
Estimates premorbid intellectual functioning on the basis of verbal ability
Perception and psychomotor speed
Line bisection test [31]
Measures unilateral neglect, which is usually a sequel of massive right hemisphere lesions
Facial recognition test [31]
Detects impairment in the discrimination of faces, a disorder associated with right hemisphere lesions
Judgment of line orientation test [31]
A test of visuospatial judgment, also detects right hemisphere dysfunction
Letter-digit substitution test (LDST) [31]
Measures psychomotor speed that is relatively unaffected by a decline in intellectual ability
Memory
Visual verbal learning test (VVLT) [31]
Examines several aspects of verbal learning, organization, and memory
Working memory task (WMT) [31]
Measures the speed of memory processes
Attention and executive function
Stroop color-word test (SCWT) [31]
Examines attention, mental speed, and mental control
Categoric word fluency task [31]
Measures the speed and flexibility of verbal thought processes
Concept shifting test (CST) [32]
Measures attention, visual searching, mental-processing speed, and the ability to mentally control simultaneous stimulus patterns

Functional/performance status

Patient’s performance status was assessed with the Karnofsky performance status scale (KPS). [28, 29] This physician-rated scale is used widely as an outcome measure in cancer clinical studies.
The capacity to carry out activities of daily living (ADL) was assessed with the Barthel Index [30]. This index consists of 10 items assessing: continence of bowels and bladder, grooming, toilet use, feeding, transfer, mobility, dressing, climbing stairs, and bathing. The items are ordered in ascending degree of difficulty. It has proven to be a reliable and valid instrument for assessing disability in basic activities of daily living (ADL) and mobility and has been used primarily with stroke patients.
Neurological functioning was rated with the neurological functional status scale developed by Order et al. [31]. Scores range from 1 to 4, with high scores for strong neurological function.

Statistical analysis

The raw scores from both the SF-36 and QLQ-BN20 were transformed to a linear scale that ranged from 0 to 100, in which a higher score represents a higher level of functioning or a worse level of symptoms [32, 33]. The following techniques were used to measure the agreement between the patient and the patient-by-proxy HRQOL scores. The mean differences (patient-by-proxy minus patient) on the SF-36 and QLQ-BN20 were summarized as means and SD. Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to test the differences between patient and patient-by-proxy scores. The proportion of subjects whose difference between patient and patient-by-proxy scores was within ±0, 5 and 10 units [34, 35] was summarized. A difference within ±0 was considered as perfect agreement. Bland–Altman limits of agreement (LA) [16, 3639] were created to measure the agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy scores and to demonstrate the extent of rater disagreement across the range of a scale (i.e., to evaluate the magnitude of disagreement, the identification of outliers, and the observation of any bias) for each method of measurement. The Bland–Altman method depicts the mean difference between two methods of measurement (the ‘bias’), and 95 % limits of agreement (prediction interval) as the mean difference (2 SD) [or more precisely (1.96 SD)]. It is the pattern of the data points that identify agreement, types of bias and outliers. It is expected that the 95 % limits include 95 % of the data point differences between the two measurements, i.e., about 95 % of the points should lie with the interval. Lin’s concordance correlation coefficient (CCC) [40, 41] was also computed for patient and patient-by-proxy scores.
To investigate the effect of neurocognitive function on the patient–proxy agreement, we defined patients as being cognitively impaired (n = 66) or cognitively intact (n = 129) based on their neuropsychological performance. We then assessed the level of agreement via LA for PCS and MCS and mean difference for both the SF-36 and QLQ-BN20 scales.

Results

In this nationwide study [18] into HRQOL and neurocognitive functioning in low-grade glioma patients, we recruited 281 adult patients with supratentorial low-grade gliomas who were asked to complete the SF-36 and QLQ-BN20 scales. Their proxies were asked to complete the questionnaire on the patient’s SF-36 and QLQ-BN20; 35 of the 281 glioma patients (12.5 %) had no patient-by-proxy assessment and were thus excluded from the analyses. Forty-four of the 239 glioma patients (18 %) who met the inclusion criteria and who were asked to participate in the neurocognitive part of the study declined to participate; the main reasons being that participation was too burdensome, or that they were reluctant to be confronted with what they believed to be a cured illness. Neurocognitive data were available for 195 patients (87.5 %), of whom 104 (53 %) had received radiotherapy 1–22 years previously. Ninety-three percent of glioma patients were tested at home; the remainder were tested in the hospital. The clinical characteristics of the LGG patients have been described previously [18]. Briefly, Table 2 shows the sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the LGG patients. The mean age was 42 years, with the majority being men (62 %). The histological diagnosis was astrocytoma (71 %), oligodendroglioma (22 %), and oligoastrocytoma (7 %). Patients’ performance status (KPS), their capacity to carry out activities of daily living (Barthel), and neurological functioning (Order) reached near-optimal levels.
Table 2
Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics
Characteristics
LGG patients (n = 195)
Age in years, mean (SD)
40.79 (11.62)
Male sex, n (%)
120 (61.54 %)
Level of education, mean (SD)
4.16 (2.09)
Radiotherapy, n (%)
 Yes
104 (53.33 %)
 No
91 (46.67 %)
Premorbid intelligence, mean (SD)
 Dutch adult reading test
99.94 (10.78)
Histological diagnosis, n (%)
 Astrocytoma
139 (71.28 %)
 Oligodendroglioma
43 (22.05 %)
 Oligoastrocytoma
13 (6.67 %)
Tumor lateralization, n (%)
 
 Left-sided
97 (49.74 %)
 Right-sided
91 (46.67 %)
 Bilateral
7 (3.59 %)
Neurosurgical intervention, n (%)
 Biopsy
84 (43.08 %)
 Resection
111 (56.92 %)
Epileptic seizures, n (%)
167 (85.64 %)
Antiepileptic drug use, n (%)
139 (71.28 %)
Years since diagnosis, mean (SD)
5.62 (3.66)
Functional/performance status, mean (SD)
 Karnofsky
88.11 (13.59)
 Barthel
19.70 (1.24)
Order
3.89 (0.35)
SD standard deviation

Agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy measurements

Table 3 summarizes the HRQOL measures for both the patients and patient-by-proxy (n = 246). Patients and patient-by-proxy scored similar on all scales, except for the SF-36 scale physical functioning and general health, and the QLQ-BN20 subscales visual disorder and communication deficit, and the single-item itchy skin, with patients reporting worse level of symptoms and better level of functioning than their proxies. There was also a statistically significant difference in the SF-36 PCS (−1.30; p = 0.02) with patients reporting a higher score than their proxies. The difference between patients and patient-by-proxy was calculated, and the proportion within ±0 (perfect agreement), 5, and 10 units was summarized with a range of 8.54 % (general health and mental health) to 76.83 % (hair loss), 19.51 % (vitality rating) to 84.55 % (hair loss), and 40.65 % (general health) to 86.59 % (role-emotional), respectively. The Bland–Altman limit of agreement (LA) for the PCS and MCS and each of the HRQOL measures revealed a fairly high agreement between the patient and patient-by-proxy rating in all HRQOL domains. The best agreement between the patient and patient-by-proxy for the SF-36 was for role-physical (points within the 95 % limit of agreement = 99.15 %, 95 % LA; −20.49–19.00) and role-emotional (99.15 %; −16.86–16.93), and for the QLQ-BN20, hair loss (98.68 %; −36.04–34.87) and bladder control (98.24 %; −43.68–38.98). A slightly poorer agreement was observed for the PCS (93.06 %; −13.63–11.03) and physical functioning (94.98 %; −31.52–25.86). Bland–Altman plots are shown for SF-36 role-physical (see Supplementary Figure S1), role-emotional (see Supplementary Figure S2), and physical functioning (see Supplementary Figure S3) to depict the extent of rater agreement across a scale range. Finally, Lin’s CCC was calculated for each HRQOL measure (Table 3). Lin’s CCC showed a moderate-to-strong relationship ranging from r = 0.37 (weakness in the legs) to r = 0.80 (physical functioning), with 79 % of the measurements greater than 0.5 [42]. The CCC for PCS was (r = 0.69) and MCS was (r = 0.55).
Table 3
Agreement of patient and patient-by-proxy ratings for the SF-36 and QLQ-BN20
SF-36a
Mean proxy
Mean patient
Mean difference (SD)
% Points within the 95% limit of agreement (LL–UL)
% Within 0 points
% Within 5 points
% Within 10 points
CCC (95 % CI)
p valuesb
PCS
44.11
45.57
−1.30 (6.16)
93.06 (−13.63–11.03)
76.42
93.09
0.69 (0.62–0.76)
0.02
MCS
41.89
42.01
0.22 (7.68)
97.11 (−15.14–15.58)
65.85
87.40
0.55 (0.44–0.65)
0.83
Physical functioning
80.08
83.44
−2.83 (14.35)
94.98 (−31.52–25.86)
32.93
67.07
77.64
0.80 (0.76–0.85)
0.01
Mental health
70.21
71.83
−1.06 (18.12)
95.68 (−37.30–35.17)
8.54
45.53
63.01
0.52 (0.42–0.62)
0.55
General health
56.08
60.16
−3.47 (22.03)
97.46 (−47.53–40.60)
8.54
26.02
40.65
0.54 (0.45–0.63)
0.04
Role-physical
15.29
16.20
−0.75 (9.87)
99.15 (−20.49–19.00)
43.90
47.97
74.39
0.52 (0.43–0.61)
0.29
Bodily pain
79.16
81.13
−1.72 (22.01)
96.68 (−45.74–42.29)
41.06
44.72
51.63
0.57 (0.49–0.66)
0.34
Vitality rating
58.94
61.54
−2.53 (20.74)
95.74 (−44.01–38.98)
10.57
19.51
41.46
0.58 (0.50–0.67)
0.14
Role-emotional
19.02
18.93
0.03 (8.45)
99.15 (−16.86–16.93)
62.60
66.67
86.59
0.58 (0.50–0.66)
0.76
Social functioning
78.41
77.35
1.17 (22.08)
96.69 (−42.99–45.33)
39.43
41.06
41.06
0.56 (0.48–0.65)
0.34
EORTC Brain Cancer module QLQ-BN20 c
Future uncertainty
25.52
23.47
2.11 (24.69)
95.18 (−47.28–51.49)
22.76
30.08
54.88
0.47 (0.38–0.57)
0.49
Visual disorder
11.28
14.08
−2.50 (19.22)
96.92 (−40.93–35.94)
45.93
53.66
53.66
0.50 (0.40–0.60)
0.04
Motor dysfunction
14.36
13.79
0.66 (18.05)
95.63 (−35.45–36.76)
47.56
54.47
54.88
0.56 (0.48–0.65)
0.48
Communication deficit
20.86
22.96
−2.26 (23.98)
95.67 (−50.22–45.70)
74.80
82.52
82.52
0.53 (0.44–0.62)
0.03
Headaches
26.75
24.620
1.90 (26.39)
97.37 (−50.22–54.68)
57.72
65.04
65.04
0.61 (0.53–0.69)
0.50
Seizures
18.03
18.63
0.15 (23.41)
97.79 (−46.68–46.97)
68.29
76.42
76.42
0.67 (0.60–0.74)
0.88
Drowsiness
23.79
21.72
1.60 (28.65)
97.82 (−55.71–58.91)
52.85
59.76
59.76
0.47 (0.37–0.57)
0.46
Hair loss
6.01
6.94
−0.59 (17.73)
98.68 (−36.04–34.87)
76.83
84.55
84.55
0.49 (0.39–0.59)
0.50
Itchy skin
9.05
11.67
−2.95 (20.39)
98.23 (−43.74–37.84)
71.95
80.08
80.08
0.59 (0.51–0.67)
0.04
Weakness legs
5.80
6.78
−1.19 (20.38)
98.22 (−41.94–39.57)
75.61
84.15
84.15
0.37 (0.25–0.48)
0.35
Bladder control
8.44
10.56
−2.35 (20.67)
98.24 (−43.68–38.98)
74.80
82.52
82.52
0.56 (0.47–0.65)
0.06
PCS Physical component summary
MCS Mental component summary
CCC Concordance correlation coefficient
aHigher scores indicate better health
bWilcoxon signed-rank test
cHigher scores indicate more of the symptoms

Impact of neurocognitive deficits on patient and patient-by-proxy agreement

The impact of neurocognitive deficits on the agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy HRQOL scores was also examined. Out of the 195 patients who had data on neurocognitive functioning, 66 (33.85 %) patients were cognitively impaired according to our definition. The mean difference in the cognitively intact patient group was overall smaller as compared to the cognitively impaired patients (Tables 4, 5), and the Bland–Altman LA was also higher in the cognitively intact group. In the cognitively impaired group, large and statistically significant differences were observed for the QLQ-BN20 visual disorder (mean difference = −7.80; p = 0.001), headaches (−5.95; p = 0.02), itchy skin (−7.02; p = 0.02), and bladder control (−8.77; p = 0.02), indicating that cognitively impaired patients and their proxies did not agree on these scales. The difference for SF-36 physical functioning was borderline significant (−4.10; p = 0.05) (Table 4). As shown in Table 5, there were no statistically significant differences in any of the HRQOL scales in the cognitively intact patient group. The largest difference was observed in the QLQ-BN20 headache (5.50; p = 0.06) with borderline significance. The LA for PCS and MCS are shown in Figs. 1 and 2 to illustrate the extent of rater agreement across the scale range in the summarized SF-36 scales.
Table 4
Mean difference (patient-by-proxy minus patient) cognitively impaired patients (n = 66)
SF-36
Mean proxy
Mean patient
Mean difference
Mean (SD)
p valuesa
Physical component summary
40.46
42.26
−1.73 (7.10)
0.07
Mental component summary
40.52
39.75
1.41 (7.90)
0.26
Physical functioning
68.64
74.03
−4.10 (17.11)
0.05
Mental health
67.22
65.31
3.06 (17.30)
0.26
General health
47.32
51.77
−3.80 (22.69)
0.23
Role-physical
10.75
12.12
−1.29 (10.27)
0.33
Bodily pain
72.62
71.10
1.52 (21.76)
0.30
Vitality
50.38
52.34
−2.57 (20.41)
0.30
Role-emotional
15.70
15.17
0.37 (10.03)
0.63
Social functioning
71.07
68.68
2.38 (22.07)
0.23
EORTC QLQ-BN20
Future uncertainty
29.66
31.63
−1.69 (21.42)
0.64
Visual disorder
15.44
23.95
−7.80 (18.89)
0.001
Motor dysfunction
22.60
23.08
0.10 (22.68)
0.80
Communication deficit
27.12
31.99
−4.87 (21.62)
0.08
Headaches
25.29
32.18
−5.95 (22.12)
0.02
Seizures
25.86
29.17
−1.82 (24.36)
0.38
Drowsiness
33.90
30.46
3.51 (27.95)
0.50
Hair loss
5.75
6.90
−1.19 (19.03)
0.59
Itchy skin
7.35
14.37
−7.02 (20.64)
0.02
Weakness legs
9.20
12.64
−4.76 (22.41)
0.18
Bladder control
12.43
21.26
−8.77 (24.01)
0.02
aWilcoxon signed-rank test
Table 5
Mean difference (patient-by-proxy minus patient) cognitively intact patients (n = 129)
SF-36 Scores
Mean proxy
Mean patient
Mean difference
Mean (SD)
p valuesa
Physical Component Summary
47.03
47.70
−0.79 (6.19)
0.77
Mental Component Summary
43.52
43.86
−0.04 (7.52)
0.66
Physical Functioning
87.98
89.82
−1.79 (13.73)
0.32
Mental health
74.07
76.07
−1.41 (18.33)
0.52
General health
63.79
65.67
−1.44 (20.90)
0.82
Role-physical
18.42
18.52
0.09 (9.85)
0.81
Bodily pain
83.53
85.91
−2.18 (24.22)
0.36
Vitality
65.07
66.72
−1.85 (21.28)
0.53
Role-emotional
21.65
20.60
1.03 (7.06)
0.11
Social functioning
85.16
84.10
1.02 (21.54)
0.64
EORTC QLQ-BN20 scores
Future uncertainty
22.06
19.87
2.26 (25.07)
0.67
Visual disorder
9.26
10.96
−1.71 (20.96)
0.22
Motor dysfunction
11.00
8.88
2.23 (15.87)
0.08
Communication deficit
17.78
19.84
−2.14 (25.96)
0.29
Headaches
26.60
21.13
5.50 (28.43)
0.06
Seizures
14.89
13.81
1.98 (23.49)
0.41
Drowsiness
18.77
17.26
0.98 (29.09)
0.75
Hair loss
7.05
7.81
−0.33 (17.86)
0.87
Itchy skin
9.80
9.01
0.67 (19.52)
0.72
Weakness legs
4.25
4.46
\( \cong \)0 (22.11)
0.99
Bladder control
6.09
5.95
−0.32 (19.52)
0.69
aWilcoxon signed-rank test

Discussion

Measuring neurocognitive functioning is essential in brain tumor patients, because this may strongly influence their HRQOL and also patient–proxy concordance levels. Although the patient is the primary source of information when measuring HRQOL, the information collected from patients with glioma may be unreliable, especially in those patients who are experiencing significant neurocognitive deterioration [14]. It is recommended to obtain proxy (i.e., caregivers) HRQOL ratings alongside a patient’s own self-report and to consider substituting patient-by-proxy ratings when a patient’s self-report is absent [43]. The original analysis of this study showed that glioma patients reported lower levels of self-reported neurocognitive functioning as measured by the MOS scales than did the healthy controls: 47.80 versus 82.40, respectively [18].
In the present study, we found that there was overall a high agreement between the patient and patient-by-proxy rating of LGG patients HRQOL in most subscales of the SF-36 and QLQ-BN20. The only statistically significant differences were observed in SF-36 physical functioning and general health and QLQ-BN20 visual disorder, communication deficit, and itchy skin. Noticeable mean differences were observed in the cognitively impaired group especially on the QLQ-BN20 scores regarding visual disorder, headaches, itchy skin, and bladder control. A noticeable difference in the cognitively intact patient group was only observed in the QLQ-BN20 headaches score. The difference between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings found in the whole group of patients could be due to the cognitively impaired patients. Although all of the differences were statistically significant, they were less than the 10 points generally accepted as clinically meaningful. However, some scores may have represented small, potentially noticeable changes in the range of 5–10 points [44] which could be important to the individual patient and warrant clinical attention.
The Bland–Altman plot revealed a high agreement between the patient and patient-by-proxy rating of HRQOL, where about 95 % of the differences between the two measurements were within the 95 % limits of agreement (prediction interval) except for SF-36 physical functioning scale and the summary component score for physical functioning (PCS). However, the limit of agreement was lower in the cognitively impaired patient group. One of the reasons for this lower agreement may be that patients, who are aware of the fact that their cognitive functions are severely affected, regard their HRQOL as poor (which is also the case in AD patients). Proxies may not fully appreciate the emotions which accompany decline of intellectual functioning.
It is important that the extent of agreement across the range of measurement be stable between the patient and patient-by-proxy [39]. Our findings showed that the agreement was poor for the central section of the scales (supplementary Figures). This was also shown by Giesinger et al. [12] who likewise stated that the possible discrepancies (i.e., ‘bias’) between the patient and patient-by-proxy are reduced by the limited range scale. The current study found a moderate-to-strong correlation between patient and patient-by-proxy scores (CCCs >0.5 for 79 % of the measurements). It is thus much easier to demonstrate agreement when a patient is experiencing either very few or many symptoms, but as the number of symptoms moves closer to 50–50 distribution, patient–proxy agreement decreases.
Regarding the methodology, our results compare quite favorably with other studies that have examined proxy ratings for general cancer patients [13, 45], brain cancer patients [12, 13], epilepsy patients [46], and stroke patients [47]. For example, a previous study in HRQOL of brain cancer patients and their proxy raters showed that intra-class correlations (ICCs) were greater than 0.5 for 77 % multi-item measures and 38 % of single-item measures [13]. However, in this previous study [13], the authors did not implement an extensive testing of patients’ neurocognitive functioning, which we did in this study. Also, the use of sound statistical techniques such as the Bland–Altman limits of agreement [16, 3639], which are straightforward and easy to interpret, enabled us to investigate any possible relationship between the measurement error and the true value. Furthermore, our study has a large sample size and a homogeneous patient population.
Patient-by-proxy ratings may resolve compliance issues when assessing HRQOL in glioma patients with intact neurocognitive function. Probably more important in glioma patients than in any other cancer patient population, but comparable to other patients with neurological diseases associated with neurocognitive decline (e.g., Mild Cognitive Impairment and AD), patient-by-proxy ratings might also be helpful when patients cognitively deteriorate and lack the ability and insight to accurately interpret and understand the HRQOL measures. In the current study, although there was a good agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings for the whole sample, there was less agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings for those patients with impaired neurocognitive function compared to those patients with unimpaired neurocognitive function. While patient and patient-by-proxy ratings in such situations should not be regarded a priori as incorrect [46], insight is needed into the sources of variation between patient and proxy ratings. In a small study that compared HRQOL ratings from proxies and patients with mild AD, mild cognitive impairment, and elderly controls, it was found that overall patient–proxy agreement did not differ significantly between groups despite evident differences in neurocognitive functioning [48]. In a related study, patients with early AD generally reported a higher HRQOL than their proxies, and discrepancies in patient–proxy ratings were associated with the presence of anosognosia [49]. Although in the current study self-awareness was not evaluated, we found that LGG patients with cognitive deficits tended to report more tumor- and treatment-related symptoms and thus a lower HRQOL. This might indicate that potentially reduced self-awareness can be associated with both higher and lower patient HRQOL ratings relative to proxy ratings. There is currently no consensus on the best way to deal with inconsistent patient–proxy reports. While most methods rely on proxy report as a ‘gold standard’ with patient–proxy concordance taken as an indirect measure of patient (lack of) insight, the accuracy of proxy reports bares critical examination when the proxy is the caregiver. While the proxy-related factors affecting patient–proxy discrepancies are largely unknown in brain tumor patients, studies in patients with mild cognitive impairment as a prodromal phase of AD, for instance, have shown that caregivers’ cognitive skills and educational level are significant predictors of the discrepancies between caregiver ADL reports and directly assessed patient performance on ADL [50]. Furthermore, caregivers’ age, financial situation and valuation of life as a whole [51], the type of caregiver, the perspective used [52], caregiver burden [53], level of depression and anxiety [54], and caregiver health may influence the accuracy of the caregiver report. As stated earlier, patient-related factors that might affect concordance between glioma patient and proxy ratings include compromised mood and decreased neurocognitive functioning [13, 14]. Interestingly, a study that focused on screening for major depressive disorder in glioma patients [55] did not find patient–proxy agreement to be associated with severity of patient cognitive impairment, although there was frequent disagreement between glioma patients and proxies reports of depressive symptoms. A study that focused on the effect of neurocognitive functioning and performance status (KPS) on patient–proxy concordance [56] found patients and proxies to have highly congruent assessments of symptom severity regardless of patients’ neurocognitive functioning and performance status. Use of proxies as a substitute for the patient self-report of HRQOL should thus be treated with caution, always taking into consideration the possibility of potential bias.
A limitation of our study is the cross-sectional nature of the data as opposed to longitudinal data or follow-up data and generally mild neurocognitive problems in LGG patients which did not allow the detection of small mean differences between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings. Follow-up data or assessment in high-grade glioma patients with probably more neurocognitive problems might have allowed the detection of small differences between patient and patient-by-proxy HRQOL ratings. The percentage of mean differences (equal or below 0, 5, and 10 points) could be impacted by the number of possible scores on a scale [12]. A very low number of possible scores or a very large distance between two possible scores (i.e., >10 points) could distort the agreement accuracy. Also, since patients in this study had stable disease with mild neurocognitive impairment, stable LGG are not representative of the general brain tumor patient population. Investigating agreement on high-grade glioma (HGG) patients with severe neurocognitive impairment would provide additional information to assess agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings. The present study specifically addressed HRQOL and did not include estimates of mood or depression. Theoretically, mood might have affected our outcomes to a certain extent as a study among patients with major depression showed that responses to self-report questionnaires are influenced by the presence of depression [57].
One issue of potential concern is basing the analyses on 195 patients from the original sample of 281 patients might result in bias. This might have been the case if patients who did not participate in neurocognitive testing were excluded, for instance, because of poor neurological or physical status. We would argue, however, that it is unlikely that bias was introduced in our study. At the time that we conducted the original study, our expectation was that only approximately 100 LGG patients would be alive in the Netherlands and meet our eligibility criteria. In fact, we were ultimately successful in identifying 281 eligible patients. Because of limited financial and personnel resources, we were only able to assess neurocognitive functioning and HRQOL in 195 consecutive patients; we assessed HRQOL only in the remaining 86 patients. There was no evidence to suggest that those who underwent both neurocognitive testing and completed HRQOL assessment differed in any significant way from those who only completed the questionnaires.
In conclusion, our data demonstrate that there is an overall high level of agreement between patient and patient-by-proxy ratings of LGG patients’ HRQOL, although the agreement for some measures is weaker in those cases where patients have neurocognitive impairment. This implies that in general, patient-by-proxy-reported outcomes can be used to replace missing patient-reported outcomes to solve compliance issues in clinical trials in this patient population. This is not the case, however, for patients with cognitive deficits who are no longer able or willing to provide self-reported data. Specifically, regarding the lack of a ‘gold standard,’ discordant patient–proxy reports should currently be considered as a parallel source of information on patient functioning. Since it is not always possible to predict which patients will suffer from progressive neurocognitive deficits, or when, it is advisable to build proxy assessments into study designs from the start of brain tumor clinical trials as is currently the case in EORTC studies 26101 (NCT01290939) and 26091 (NCT01164189).

Acknowledgments

We thank the neurologists, neuro-oncologists, neurosurgeons, and radiation oncologists from VU University Medical Center, The Netherlands Cancer Institute/Antoni van Leeuwenhoek Hospital, Slotervaart Hospital, Academic Medical Center (Amsterdam), Leiden University Medical Center (Leiden); Medical Center Haaglanden, HAGA Hospital (The Hague); Erasmus University Medical Center (Rotterdam); St. Elisabeth Hospital (Tilburg); University Medical Center Maastricht (Maastricht); Hospital De Wever (Heerlen); University Medical Center Utrecht (Utrecht); Sint Antonius Hospital (Nieuwegein); University Medical Center Sint Radboud (Nijmegen); and Groningen University Medical Center, Martini Hospital (Groningen) for permission to include their patient–proxy dyads in the study. Furthermore, we would like to thank the Dutch Cancer Society, La Fondation Contre le Cancer, National Cancer Institute, EORTC Research Fund, and Pfizer Global Partnership Foundation for funding this study; and Cheryl Whittaker for editorial help.

Funding

This work was supported by Grant VU96-1155 from the Dutch Cancer Society and by grants from La Fondation Contre le Cancer and USA Grant No 5U10 CA011488-39 through 5U10 CA011488-40 from National Cancer Institute (Bethesda, MD), European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer (EORTC) Charitable Trust, and Pfizer Global Partnership Foundation.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflicts of interest

No authors have a potential conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Podotherapeut Totaal

Binnen de bundel kunt u gebruik maken van boeken, tijdschriften, e-learnings, web-tv's en uitlegvideo's. BSL Podotherapeut Totaal is overal toegankelijk; via uw PC, tablet of smartphone.

Bijlagen

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Taphoorn, M. J., Stupp, R., Coens, C., Osoba, D., Kortmann, R., van den Bent, M. J., et al. (2005). Health-related quality of life in patients with glioblastoma: A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Oncology, 6(12), 937–944.CrossRefPubMed Taphoorn, M. J., Stupp, R., Coens, C., Osoba, D., Kortmann, R., van den Bent, M. J., et al. (2005). Health-related quality of life in patients with glioblastoma: A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Oncology, 6(12), 937–944.CrossRefPubMed
2.
go back to reference Taphoorn, M. J., van den Bent, M. J., Mauer, M. E., Coens, C., Delattre, J. Y., Brandes, A. A., et al. (2007). Health-related quality of life in patients treated for anaplastic oligodendroglioma with adjuvant chemotherapy: Results of a European Organisation for Research and Treatment of Cancer randomized clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 25(36), 5723–5730.CrossRefPubMed Taphoorn, M. J., van den Bent, M. J., Mauer, M. E., Coens, C., Delattre, J. Y., Brandes, A. A., et al. (2007). Health-related quality of life in patients treated for anaplastic oligodendroglioma with adjuvant chemotherapy: Results of a European Organisation for Research and Treatment of Cancer randomized clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 25(36), 5723–5730.CrossRefPubMed
3.
go back to reference Reardon, D. A., Galanis, E., DeGroot, J. F., Cloughesy, T. F., Wefel, J. S., Lamborn, K. R., et al. (2011). Clinical trial end points for high-grade glioma: The evolving landscape. Neuro-Oncology, 13(3), 353–361.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Reardon, D. A., Galanis, E., DeGroot, J. F., Cloughesy, T. F., Wefel, J. S., Lamborn, K. R., et al. (2011). Clinical trial end points for high-grade glioma: The evolving landscape. Neuro-Oncology, 13(3), 353–361.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
go back to reference Reijneveld, J. C., Sitskoorn, M. M., Klein, M., Nuyen, J., & Taphoorn, M. J. (2001). Cognitive status and quality of life in patients with suspected versus proven low-grade gliomas. Neurology, 56(5), 618–623.CrossRefPubMed Reijneveld, J. C., Sitskoorn, M. M., Klein, M., Nuyen, J., & Taphoorn, M. J. (2001). Cognitive status and quality of life in patients with suspected versus proven low-grade gliomas. Neurology, 56(5), 618–623.CrossRefPubMed
5.
go back to reference Taphoorn, M. J., Sizoo, E. M., & Bottomley, A. (2010). Review on quality of life issues in patients with primary brain tumors. The Oncologist, 15(6), 618–626.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Taphoorn, M. J., Sizoo, E. M., & Bottomley, A. (2010). Review on quality of life issues in patients with primary brain tumors. The Oncologist, 15(6), 618–626.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
go back to reference Klein, M., Engelberts, N. H., van der Ploeg, H. M., Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D. G., Aaronson, N. K., Taphoorn, M. J., et al. (2003). Epilepsy in low-grade gliomas: The impact on cognitive function and quality of life. Annals of Neurology, 54(4), 514–520.CrossRefPubMed Klein, M., Engelberts, N. H., van der Ploeg, H. M., Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D. G., Aaronson, N. K., Taphoorn, M. J., et al. (2003). Epilepsy in low-grade gliomas: The impact on cognitive function and quality of life. Annals of Neurology, 54(4), 514–520.CrossRefPubMed
7.
go back to reference Struik, K., Klein, M., Heimans, J. J., Gielissen, M. F., Bleijenberg, G., Taphoorn, M. J., et al. (2009). Fatigue in low-grade glioma. Journal of Neuro-Oncology, 92(1), 73–78.CrossRefPubMed Struik, K., Klein, M., Heimans, J. J., Gielissen, M. F., Bleijenberg, G., Taphoorn, M. J., et al. (2009). Fatigue in low-grade glioma. Journal of Neuro-Oncology, 92(1), 73–78.CrossRefPubMed
8.
go back to reference Mainio, A., Hakko, H., Niemela, A., Koivukangas, J., & Rasanen, P. (2005). Depression and functional outcome in patients with brain tumors: A population-based 1-year follow-up study. Journal of Neurosurgery, 103(5), 841–847.CrossRefPubMed Mainio, A., Hakko, H., Niemela, A., Koivukangas, J., & Rasanen, P. (2005). Depression and functional outcome in patients with brain tumors: A population-based 1-year follow-up study. Journal of Neurosurgery, 103(5), 841–847.CrossRefPubMed
9.
go back to reference Boele, F. W., Zant, M., Heine, E. C., Aaronson, N. K., Taphoorn, M. J., Reijneveld, J. C., et al. (2014). The association between cognitive functioning and health-related quality of life in low-grade glioma patients. Neuro-Oncology Practice, 1(2), 40–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Boele, F. W., Zant, M., Heine, E. C., Aaronson, N. K., Taphoorn, M. J., Reijneveld, J. C., et al. (2014). The association between cognitive functioning and health-related quality of life in low-grade glioma patients. Neuro-Oncology Practice, 1(2), 40–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
go back to reference Taphoorn, M. J., & Klein, M. (2004). Cognitive deficits in adult patients with brain tumours. The Lancet Neurology, 3(3), 159–168.CrossRefPubMed Taphoorn, M. J., & Klein, M. (2004). Cognitive deficits in adult patients with brain tumours. The Lancet Neurology, 3(3), 159–168.CrossRefPubMed
11.
go back to reference Sizoo, E. M., Braam, L., Postma, T. J., Pasman, H. R., Heimans, J. J., Klein, M., et al. (2010). Symptoms and problems in the end-of-life phase of high-grade glioma patients. Neuro-Oncology, 12(11), 1162–1166.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sizoo, E. M., Braam, L., Postma, T. J., Pasman, H. R., Heimans, J. J., Klein, M., et al. (2010). Symptoms and problems in the end-of-life phase of high-grade glioma patients. Neuro-Oncology, 12(11), 1162–1166.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
go back to reference Giesinger, J. M., Golser, M., Erharter, A., Kemmler, G., Schauer-Maurer, G., Stockhammer, G., et al. (2009). Do neurooncological patients and their significant others agree on quality of life ratings? Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 7, 87.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Giesinger, J. M., Golser, M., Erharter, A., Kemmler, G., Schauer-Maurer, G., Stockhammer, G., et al. (2009). Do neurooncological patients and their significant others agree on quality of life ratings? Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 7, 87.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
go back to reference Sneeuw, K. C., Aaronson, N. K., Osoba, D., Muller, M. J., Hsu, M. A., Yung, W. K., et al. (1997). The use of significant others as proxy raters of the quality of life of patients with brain cancer. Medical Care, 35(5), 490–506.CrossRefPubMed Sneeuw, K. C., Aaronson, N. K., Osoba, D., Muller, M. J., Hsu, M. A., Yung, W. K., et al. (1997). The use of significant others as proxy raters of the quality of life of patients with brain cancer. Medical Care, 35(5), 490–506.CrossRefPubMed
14.
go back to reference Brown, P. D., Decker, P. A., Rummans, T. A., Clark, M. M., Frost, M. H., Ballman, K. V., et al. (2008). A prospective study of quality of life in adults with newly diagnosed high-grade gliomas: Comparison of patient and caregiver ratings of quality of life. American Journal of Clinical Oncology, 31(2), 163–168.CrossRefPubMed Brown, P. D., Decker, P. A., Rummans, T. A., Clark, M. M., Frost, M. H., Ballman, K. V., et al. (2008). A prospective study of quality of life in adults with newly diagnosed high-grade gliomas: Comparison of patient and caregiver ratings of quality of life. American Journal of Clinical Oncology, 31(2), 163–168.CrossRefPubMed
15.
go back to reference Sneeuw, K. C., Aaronson, N. K., Sprangers, M. A., Detmar, S. B., Wever, L. D., & Schornagel, J. H. (1997). Value of caregiver ratings in evaluating the quality of life of patients with cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 15(3), 1206–1217.CrossRefPubMed Sneeuw, K. C., Aaronson, N. K., Sprangers, M. A., Detmar, S. B., Wever, L. D., & Schornagel, J. H. (1997). Value of caregiver ratings in evaluating the quality of life of patients with cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 15(3), 1206–1217.CrossRefPubMed
16.
go back to reference Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1986). Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. The Lancet, 1(8476), 307–310.CrossRef Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1986). Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. The Lancet, 1(8476), 307–310.CrossRef
17.
go back to reference Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1990). A note on the use of the intraclass correlation coefficient in the evaluation of agreement between two methods of measurement. Computers in Biology and Medicine, 20(5), 337–340.CrossRefPubMed Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1990). A note on the use of the intraclass correlation coefficient in the evaluation of agreement between two methods of measurement. Computers in Biology and Medicine, 20(5), 337–340.CrossRefPubMed
18.
go back to reference Klein, M., Heimans, J. J., Aaronson, N. K., van der Ploeg, H. M., Grit, J., Muller, M., et al. (2002). Effect of radiotherapy and other treatment-related factors on mid-term to long-term cognitive sequelae in low-grade gliomas: A comparative study. The Lancet, 360(9343), 1361–1368.CrossRef Klein, M., Heimans, J. J., Aaronson, N. K., van der Ploeg, H. M., Grit, J., Muller, M., et al. (2002). Effect of radiotherapy and other treatment-related factors on mid-term to long-term cognitive sequelae in low-grade gliomas: A comparative study. The Lancet, 360(9343), 1361–1368.CrossRef
19.
go back to reference Ware, J. E., Jr., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.CrossRefPubMed Ware, J. E., Jr., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.CrossRefPubMed
20.
go back to reference Stewart, A. L., & Ware, J. E. (Eds.). (1992). Measuring functioning and well-being: The medical outcomes study approach. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Stewart, A. L., & Ware, J. E. (Eds.). (1992). Measuring functioning and well-being: The medical outcomes study approach. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
21.
go back to reference Osoba, D., Aaronson, N. K., Muller, M., Sneeuw, K., Hsu, M. A., Yung, W. K., et al. (1996). The development and psychometric validation of a brain cancer quality-of-life questionnaire for use in combination with general cancer-specific questionnaires. Quality of Life Research, 5(1), 139–150.CrossRefPubMed Osoba, D., Aaronson, N. K., Muller, M., Sneeuw, K., Hsu, M. A., Yung, W. K., et al. (1996). The development and psychometric validation of a brain cancer quality-of-life questionnaire for use in combination with general cancer-specific questionnaires. Quality of Life Research, 5(1), 139–150.CrossRefPubMed
22.
go back to reference McHorney, C. A., Ware, J. E., Jr., & Raczek, A. E. (1993). The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): II. Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31(3), 247–263.CrossRefPubMed McHorney, C. A., Ware, J. E., Jr., & Raczek, A. E. (1993). The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): II. Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31(3), 247–263.CrossRefPubMed
23.
go back to reference Brazier, J. E., Harper, R., Jones, N. M., O’Cathain, A., Thomas, K. J., Usherwood, T., et al. (1992). Validating the SF-36 health survey questionnaire: New outcome measure for primary care. BMJ, 305(6846), 160–164.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Brazier, J. E., Harper, R., Jones, N. M., O’Cathain, A., Thomas, K. J., Usherwood, T., et al. (1992). Validating the SF-36 health survey questionnaire: New outcome measure for primary care. BMJ, 305(6846), 160–164.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
go back to reference Jenkinson, C., Coulter, A., & Wright, L. (1993). Short form 36 (SF36) health survey questionnaire: Normative data for adults of working age. BMJ, 306(6890), 1437–1440.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Jenkinson, C., Coulter, A., & Wright, L. (1993). Short form 36 (SF36) health survey questionnaire: Normative data for adults of working age. BMJ, 306(6890), 1437–1440.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
go back to reference Aaronson, N. K., Muller, M., Cohen, P. D., Essink-Bot, M. L., Fekkes, M., Sanderman, R., et al. (1998). Translation, validation, and norming of the Dutch language version of the SF-36 Health Survey in community and chronic disease populations. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 51(11), 1055–1068.CrossRefPubMed Aaronson, N. K., Muller, M., Cohen, P. D., Essink-Bot, M. L., Fekkes, M., Sanderman, R., et al. (1998). Translation, validation, and norming of the Dutch language version of the SF-36 Health Survey in community and chronic disease populations. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 51(11), 1055–1068.CrossRefPubMed
26.
go back to reference Efficace, F., & Bottomley, A. (2002). Health related quality of life assessment methodology and reported outcomes in randomised controlled trials of primary brain cancer patients. European Journal of Cancer, 38(14), 1824–1831.CrossRefPubMed Efficace, F., & Bottomley, A. (2002). Health related quality of life assessment methodology and reported outcomes in randomised controlled trials of primary brain cancer patients. European Journal of Cancer, 38(14), 1824–1831.CrossRefPubMed
27.
go back to reference van Dam, F. S., Schagen, S. B., Muller, M. J., Boogerd, W., vd Wall, E., Droogleever Fortuyn, M. E., et al. (1998). Impairment of cognitive function in women receiving adjuvant treatment for high-risk breast cancer: High-dose versus standard-dose chemotherapy. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 90(3), 210–218.CrossRefPubMed van Dam, F. S., Schagen, S. B., Muller, M. J., Boogerd, W., vd Wall, E., Droogleever Fortuyn, M. E., et al. (1998). Impairment of cognitive function in women receiving adjuvant treatment for high-risk breast cancer: High-dose versus standard-dose chemotherapy. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 90(3), 210–218.CrossRefPubMed
28.
go back to reference Karnofsky, D. A., Abelmann, W. H., & Craver, L. F. (1948). The use of nitrogen mustards in the palliative treatment of carcinoma. Cancer, 1, 634–656.CrossRef Karnofsky, D. A., Abelmann, W. H., & Craver, L. F. (1948). The use of nitrogen mustards in the palliative treatment of carcinoma. Cancer, 1, 634–656.CrossRef
29.
go back to reference Karnofsky, D. A., & Burchenal, J. H. (1949). The clinical evaluation of chemotherapeutic agents in cancer. In C. M. Macleod (Ed.), Evaluation of chemotherapeutic agents (pp. 199–205). New York: Columbia University Press. Karnofsky, D. A., & Burchenal, J. H. (1949). The clinical evaluation of chemotherapeutic agents in cancer. In C. M. Macleod (Ed.), Evaluation of chemotherapeutic agents (pp. 199–205). New York: Columbia University Press.
30.
go back to reference Wade, D. T., & Collin, C. (1988). The Barthel ADL Index: A standard measure of physical disability? International Disability Studies, 10(2), 64–67.CrossRefPubMed Wade, D. T., & Collin, C. (1988). The Barthel ADL Index: A standard measure of physical disability? International Disability Studies, 10(2), 64–67.CrossRefPubMed
31.
go back to reference Order, S. E., Hellman, S., Von Essen, C. F., & Kligerman, M. M. (1968). Improvement in quality of survival following whole-brain irradiation for brain metastasis. Radiology, 91(1), 149–153.CrossRefPubMed Order, S. E., Hellman, S., Von Essen, C. F., & Kligerman, M. M. (1968). Improvement in quality of survival following whole-brain irradiation for brain metastasis. Radiology, 91(1), 149–153.CrossRefPubMed
32.
go back to reference Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., & Dewey, J. E. (2000). How to score version 2 of the SF-36 health survey. Lincoln, RI: Quality Metric Inc. Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., & Dewey, J. E. (2000). How to score version 2 of the SF-36 health survey. Lincoln, RI: Quality Metric Inc.
33.
go back to reference Fayers, P. M., Aaronson, N. K., Bjordal, K., Groenvold, M., Curran, D., Bottomley, A., et al. (2001). EORTC QLQ-C30 scoring manual (3rd ed.). Brussels: EORTC. Fayers, P. M., Aaronson, N. K., Bjordal, K., Groenvold, M., Curran, D., Bottomley, A., et al. (2001). EORTC QLQ-C30 scoring manual (3rd ed.). Brussels: EORTC.
34.
go back to reference Osoba, D., Rodrigues, G., Myles, J., Zee, B., & Pater, J. (1998). Interpreting the significance of changes in health-related quality-of-life scores. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 16(1), 139–144.CrossRefPubMed Osoba, D., Rodrigues, G., Myles, J., Zee, B., & Pater, J. (1998). Interpreting the significance of changes in health-related quality-of-life scores. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 16(1), 139–144.CrossRefPubMed
35.
go back to reference King, M. T. (1996). The interpretation of scores from the EORTC quality of life questionnaire QLQ-C30. Quality of Life Research, 5(6), 555–567.CrossRefPubMed King, M. T. (1996). The interpretation of scores from the EORTC quality of life questionnaire QLQ-C30. Quality of Life Research, 5(6), 555–567.CrossRefPubMed
36.
go back to reference Altman, D. G., & Bland, J. M. (1983). Measurement in medicine: The analysis of method comparison studies. The Statistician, 32, 307–317.CrossRef Altman, D. G., & Bland, J. M. (1983). Measurement in medicine: The analysis of method comparison studies. The Statistician, 32, 307–317.CrossRef
37.
go back to reference Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1995). Comparing methods of measurement: Why plotting difference against standard method is misleading. The Lancet, 346(8982), 1085–1087.CrossRef Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1995). Comparing methods of measurement: Why plotting difference against standard method is misleading. The Lancet, 346(8982), 1085–1087.CrossRef
38.
go back to reference Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Applying the right statistics: Analyses of measurement studies. Ultrasound in Obstetrics and Gynecology, 22(1), 85–93.CrossRefPubMed Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Applying the right statistics: Analyses of measurement studies. Ultrasound in Obstetrics and Gynecology, 22(1), 85–93.CrossRefPubMed
39.
go back to reference Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (2007). Agreement between methods of measurement with multiple observations per individual. Journal of Biopharmaceutical Statistics, 17(4), 571–582.CrossRefPubMed Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (2007). Agreement between methods of measurement with multiple observations per individual. Journal of Biopharmaceutical Statistics, 17(4), 571–582.CrossRefPubMed
40.
go back to reference Carrasco, J. L., & Jover, L. (2003). Estimating the generalized concordance correlation coefficient through variance components. Biometrics, 59(4), 849–858.CrossRefPubMed Carrasco, J. L., & Jover, L. (2003). Estimating the generalized concordance correlation coefficient through variance components. Biometrics, 59(4), 849–858.CrossRefPubMed
41.
go back to reference Lin, L. I. (1989). A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate reproducibility. Biometrics, 45(1), 255–268.CrossRefPubMed Lin, L. I. (1989). A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate reproducibility. Biometrics, 45(1), 255–268.CrossRefPubMed
42.
go back to reference Burns, N., & Grove, S. K. (1997). The practice of nursing research conduct, critique, and utilization (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders & Co. Burns, N., & Grove, S. K. (1997). The practice of nursing research conduct, critique, and utilization (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders & Co.
43.
go back to reference Brown, P. D., Buckner, J. C., O’Fallon, J. R., Iturria, N. L., Brown, C. A., O’Neill, B. P., et al. (2003). Effects of radiotherapy on cognitive function in patients with low-grade glioma measured by the folstein mini-mental state examination. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 21(13), 2519–2524.CrossRefPubMed Brown, P. D., Buckner, J. C., O’Fallon, J. R., Iturria, N. L., Brown, C. A., O’Neill, B. P., et al. (2003). Effects of radiotherapy on cognitive function in patients with low-grade glioma measured by the folstein mini-mental state examination. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 21(13), 2519–2524.CrossRefPubMed
44.
go back to reference Ringash, J., O’Sullivan, B., Bezjak, A., & Redelmeier, D. A. (2007). Interpreting clinically significant changes in patient-reported outcomes. Cancer, 110(1), 196–202.CrossRefPubMed Ringash, J., O’Sullivan, B., Bezjak, A., & Redelmeier, D. A. (2007). Interpreting clinically significant changes in patient-reported outcomes. Cancer, 110(1), 196–202.CrossRefPubMed
45.
go back to reference Akin, S., & Durna, Z. (2013). A comparative descriptive study examining the perceptions of cancer patients, family caregivers, and nurses on patient symptom severity in Turkey. European Journal of Oncology Nursing, 17(1), 30–37.CrossRefPubMed Akin, S., & Durna, Z. (2013). A comparative descriptive study examining the perceptions of cancer patients, family caregivers, and nurses on patient symptom severity in Turkey. European Journal of Oncology Nursing, 17(1), 30–37.CrossRefPubMed
46.
go back to reference Hays, R. D., Vickrey, B. G., Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., Cramer, J., Meador, K., et al. (1995). Agreement between self reports and proxy reports of quality of life in epilepsy patients. Quality of Life Research, 4(2), 159–168.CrossRefPubMed Hays, R. D., Vickrey, B. G., Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., Cramer, J., Meador, K., et al. (1995). Agreement between self reports and proxy reports of quality of life in epilepsy patients. Quality of Life Research, 4(2), 159–168.CrossRefPubMed
47.
go back to reference Sneeuw, K. C., Aaronson, N. K., de Haan, R. J., & Limburg, M. (1997). Assessing quality of life after stroke. The value and limitations of proxy ratings. Stroke, 28(8), 1541–1549.CrossRefPubMed Sneeuw, K. C., Aaronson, N. K., de Haan, R. J., & Limburg, M. (1997). Assessing quality of life after stroke. The value and limitations of proxy ratings. Stroke, 28(8), 1541–1549.CrossRefPubMed
48.
go back to reference Ready, R. E., Ott, B. R., & Grace, J. (2004). Patient versus informant perspectives of Quality of Life in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(3), 256–265.CrossRefPubMed Ready, R. E., Ott, B. R., & Grace, J. (2004). Patient versus informant perspectives of Quality of Life in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(3), 256–265.CrossRefPubMed
49.
go back to reference Vogel, A., Mortensen, E. L., Hasselbalch, S. G., Andersen, B. B., & Waldemar, G. (2006). Patient versus informant reported quality of life in the earliest phases of Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(12), 1132–1138.CrossRefPubMed Vogel, A., Mortensen, E. L., Hasselbalch, S. G., Andersen, B. B., & Waldemar, G. (2006). Patient versus informant reported quality of life in the earliest phases of Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(12), 1132–1138.CrossRefPubMed
50.
go back to reference Dassel, K. B., & Schmitt, F. A. (2008). The impact of caregiver executive skills on reports of patient functioning. Gerontologist, 48(6), 781–792.CrossRefPubMed Dassel, K. B., & Schmitt, F. A. (2008). The impact of caregiver executive skills on reports of patient functioning. Gerontologist, 48(6), 781–792.CrossRefPubMed
51.
go back to reference Arons, A. M., Krabbe, P. F., Scholzel-Dorenbos, C. J., van der Wilt, G. J., & Rikkert, M. G. (2013). Quality of life in dementia: A study on proxy bias. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 13, 110.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Arons, A. M., Krabbe, P. F., Scholzel-Dorenbos, C. J., van der Wilt, G. J., & Rikkert, M. G. (2013). Quality of life in dementia: A study on proxy bias. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 13, 110.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
52.
go back to reference McPhail, S., Beller, E., & Haines, T. (2008). Two perspectives of proxy reporting of health-related quality of life using the Euroqol-5D, an investigation of agreement. Medical Care, 46(11), 1140–1148.CrossRefPubMed McPhail, S., Beller, E., & Haines, T. (2008). Two perspectives of proxy reporting of health-related quality of life using the Euroqol-5D, an investigation of agreement. Medical Care, 46(11), 1140–1148.CrossRefPubMed
53.
go back to reference Sands, L. P., Ferreira, P., Stewart, A. L., Brod, M., & Yaffe, K. (2004). What explains differences between dementia patients’ and their caregivers’ ratings of patients’ quality of life? The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(3), 272–280.CrossRefPubMed Sands, L. P., Ferreira, P., Stewart, A. L., Brod, M., & Yaffe, K. (2004). What explains differences between dementia patients’ and their caregivers’ ratings of patients’ quality of life? The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(3), 272–280.CrossRefPubMed
54.
go back to reference Black, B. S., Johnston, D., Morrison, A., Rabins, P. V., Lyketsos, C. G., & Samus, Q. M. (2012). Quality of life of community-residing persons with dementia based on self-rated and caregiver-rated measures. Quality of Life Research, 21(8), 1379–1389.CrossRefPubMed Black, B. S., Johnston, D., Morrison, A., Rabins, P. V., Lyketsos, C. G., & Samus, Q. M. (2012). Quality of life of community-residing persons with dementia based on self-rated and caregiver-rated measures. Quality of Life Research, 21(8), 1379–1389.CrossRefPubMed
55.
go back to reference Rooney, A. G., McNamara, S., Mackinnon, M., Fraser, M., Rampling, R., Carson, A., et al. (2013). Screening for major depressive disorder in adults with glioma using the PHQ-9: A comparison of patient versus proxy reports. Journal of Neuro-Oncology, 113(1), 49–55.CrossRefPubMed Rooney, A. G., McNamara, S., Mackinnon, M., Fraser, M., Rampling, R., Carson, A., et al. (2013). Screening for major depressive disorder in adults with glioma using the PHQ-9: A comparison of patient versus proxy reports. Journal of Neuro-Oncology, 113(1), 49–55.CrossRefPubMed
56.
go back to reference Armstrong, T. S., Wefel, J. S., Gning, I., Acquaye, A., Vera-Bolanos, E., Gilbert, M. R., et al. (2012). Congruence of primary brain tumor patient and caregiver symptom report. Cancer, 118(20), 5026–5037.CrossRefPubMed Armstrong, T. S., Wefel, J. S., Gning, I., Acquaye, A., Vera-Bolanos, E., Gilbert, M. R., et al. (2012). Congruence of primary brain tumor patient and caregiver symptom report. Cancer, 118(20), 5026–5037.CrossRefPubMed
57.
go back to reference Papakostas, G. I., Petersen, T., Mahal, Y., Mischoulon, D., Nierenberg, A. A., & Fava, M. (2004). Quality of life assessments in major depressive disorder: A review of the literature. General Hospital Psychiatry, 26(1), 13–17.CrossRefPubMed Papakostas, G. I., Petersen, T., Mahal, Y., Mischoulon, D., Nierenberg, A. A., & Fava, M. (2004). Quality of life assessments in major depressive disorder: A review of the literature. General Hospital Psychiatry, 26(1), 13–17.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Impact of neurocognitive deficits on patient–proxy agreement regarding health-related quality of life in low-grade glioma patients
Auteurs
Divine E. Ediebah
Jaap C. Reijneveld
Martin J. B. Taphoorn
Corneel Coens
Efstathios Zikos
Neil K. Aaronson
Jan J. Heimans
Andrew Bottomley
Martin Klein
on behalf of the EORTC Quality of Life Department and Patient Reported Outcome and Behavioral Evidence (PROBE)
Publicatiedatum
15-10-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer International Publishing
Gepubliceerd in
Quality of Life Research / Uitgave 4/2017
Print ISSN: 0962-9343
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2649
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-016-1426-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2017

Quality of Life Research 4/2017 Naar de uitgave