Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2014

01-01-2014 | Original Article

Imagined own-body transformations during passive self-motion

Auteurs: Michiel van Elk, Olaf Blanke

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Spatial perspective taking is a crucial social skill that underlies many of our everyday interactions. Previous studies have suggested that spatial perspective taking is an embodied process that involves the integration of both motor and proprioceptive information. Given the importance of vestibular signals for own-body perception, mental own-body imagery, and bodily self-consciousness, in the present study we hypothesized that vestibular stimulation due to passive own-body displacements should also modulate spatial perspective taking. Participants performed an own-body transformation task while being passively rotated in a clockwise or counter-clockwise direction on a human motion platform. A congruency effect was observed, reflected in faster reaction times if the implied mental body rotation direction matched the actual rotation direction of the chair. These findings indicate that vestibular stimulation modulates and facilitates mental perspective taking, thereby highlighting the importance of integrating multisensory bodily information for spatial perspective taking.
Voetnoten
1
As different processes are involved in mental body transformations (Gardner & Potts, 2011; May & Wendt, 2012) and subjects often report different strategies (Kessler & Wang, 2012) we have decided in this manuscript to use the term "mental body transformation" instead of own body transformation.
 
2
Post hoc tests did not reveal significant differences between stimuli implying a CW vs. a CCW rotation (t(17) = 1.4, p = 0.18 for CW chair rotations, t(17) = −1.6, p = 0.14 for CCW chair rotations), indicating that although rotation direction did affect mental transformation, it did not result in a complete reversal of the directionality effects. This could be partly related to the fact of spatial compatibility effects partly underlying the MBT task (Gardner & Potts, 2011; May & Wendt, 2012) and strategy differences between participants (i.e., egocentric vs. allocentric strategy; see also below).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Amorim, M. A., Isableu, B., & Jarraya, M. (2006). Embodied spatial transformations: “body analogy” for the mental rotation of objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 327–347.CrossRef Amorim, M. A., Isableu, B., & Jarraya, M. (2006). Embodied spatial transformations: “body analogy” for the mental rotation of objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 327–347.CrossRef
go back to reference Angelaki, D. E., & Cullen, K. E. (2008). Vestibular system: the many facets of a multimodal sense. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 125–150.CrossRefPubMed Angelaki, D. E., & Cullen, K. E. (2008). Vestibular system: the many facets of a multimodal sense. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 125–150.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Arzy, S., Thut, G., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., & Blanke, O. (2006). Neural basis of embodiment: distinct contributions of temporoparietal junction and extrastriate body area. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(31), 8074–8081.CrossRefPubMed Arzy, S., Thut, G., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., & Blanke, O. (2006). Neural basis of embodiment: distinct contributions of temporoparietal junction and extrastriate body area. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(31), 8074–8081.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Avraamides, M. N., & Kelly, J. W. (2008). Multiple systems of spatial memory and action. Cognitive Processes, 9(2), 93–106.CrossRef Avraamides, M. N., & Kelly, J. W. (2008). Multiple systems of spatial memory and action. Cognitive Processes, 9(2), 93–106.CrossRef
go back to reference Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., & Motes, A. M. (2006). Object-spatial imagery: a new self-report imagery questionnaire. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 239–263.CrossRef Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., & Motes, A. M. (2006). Object-spatial imagery: a new self-report imagery questionnaire. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 239–263.CrossRef
go back to reference Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(8), 556–571.PubMed Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(8), 556–571.PubMed
go back to reference Blanke, O., Ionta, S., Fornari, E., Mohr, C., & Maeder, P. (2010). Mental imagery for full and upper human bodies: common right hemisphere activations and distinct extrastriate activations. Brain Topography, 23(3), 321–332.CrossRefPubMed Blanke, O., Ionta, S., Fornari, E., Mohr, C., & Maeder, P. (2010). Mental imagery for full and upper human bodies: common right hemisphere activations and distinct extrastriate activations. Brain Topography, 23(3), 321–332.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(3), 550–557.CrossRefPubMed Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(3), 550–557.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2003). Updating space during imagined self- and array translations. Memory and Cognition, 31(6), 941–952.CrossRefPubMed Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2003). Updating space during imagined self- and array translations. Memory and Cognition, 31(6), 941–952.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Creem-Regehr, S. H., Neil, J. A., & Yeh, H. J. (2007). Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations. Neuroimage, 35(2), 916–927.CrossRefPubMed Creem-Regehr, S. H., Neil, J. A., & Yeh, H. J. (2007). Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations. Neuroimage, 35(2), 916–927.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Easton, R. D., & Sholl, M. J. (1995). Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 483–500.CrossRefPubMed Easton, R. D., & Sholl, M. J. (1995). Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 483–500.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Falconer, C. J., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Balancing the mind. Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 332–339. Falconer, C. J., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Balancing the mind. Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 332–339.
go back to reference Ferre, E. R., Bottini, G., & Haggard, P. (2011). Vestibular modulation of somatosensory perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 34(8), 1337–1344.CrossRefPubMed Ferre, E. R., Bottini, G., & Haggard, P. (2011). Vestibular modulation of somatosensory perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 34(8), 1337–1344.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Figliozzi, F., Guariglia, P., Silvetti, M., Siegler, I., & Doricchi, F. (2005). Effects of vestibular rotatory accelerations on covert attentional orienting in vision and touch. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1638–1651.CrossRefPubMed Figliozzi, F., Guariglia, P., Silvetti, M., Siegler, I., & Doricchi, F. (2005). Effects of vestibular rotatory accelerations on covert attentional orienting in vision and touch. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1638–1651.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fourkas, A. D., Ionta, S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Influence of imagined posture and imagery modality on corticospinal excitability. Behavioral Brain Research, 168(2), 190–196.CrossRef Fourkas, A. D., Ionta, S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Influence of imagined posture and imagery modality on corticospinal excitability. Behavioral Brain Research, 168(2), 190–196.CrossRef
go back to reference Gardner, M. R., & Potts, R. (2011). Domain general mechanisms account for imagined transformations of whole body perspective. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 371–381.CrossRefPubMed Gardner, M. R., & Potts, R. (2011). Domain general mechanisms account for imagined transformations of whole body perspective. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 371–381.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1979). The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cognitive Psychology, 11(3), 375–394.CrossRefPubMed Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1979). The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cognitive Psychology, 11(3), 375–394.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ionta, S., Gassert, R., & Blanke, O. (2011a). Multi-sensory and sensorimotor foundation of bodily self-consciousness—an interdisciplinary approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 383.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Ionta, S., Gassert, R., & Blanke, O. (2011a). Multi-sensory and sensorimotor foundation of bodily self-consciousness—an interdisciplinary approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 383.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ionta, S., Heydrich, L., Lenggenhager, B., Mouthon, M., Fornari, E., Chapuis, D., et al. (2011b). Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron, 70(2), 363–374.CrossRefPubMed Ionta, S., Heydrich, L., Lenggenhager, B., Mouthon, M., Fornari, E., Chapuis, D., et al. (2011b). Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron, 70(2), 363–374.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Karnath, H. O., & Dieterich, M. (2006). Spatial neglect—a vestibular disorder? Brain, 129(Pt 2), 293–305.PubMed Karnath, H. O., & Dieterich, M. (2006). Spatial neglect—a vestibular disorder? Brain, 129(Pt 2), 293–305.PubMed
go back to reference Keehner, M., Guerin, S. A., Miller, M. B., Turk, D. J., & Hegarty, M. (2006). Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during imagined spatial transformations. Neuroimage, 33(1), 391–398.CrossRefPubMed Keehner, M., Guerin, S. A., Miller, M. B., Turk, D. J., & Hegarty, M. (2006). Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during imagined spatial transformations. Neuroimage, 33(1), 391–398.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kennedy, R. S., Lane, N. E., Berbaum, K. S., & Lilienthal, M. G. (1989). Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3(3), 203–220.CrossRef Kennedy, R. S., Lane, N. E., Berbaum, K. S., & Lilienthal, M. G. (1989). Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3(3), 203–220.CrossRef
go back to reference Kessler, K., & Rutherford, H. (2010). The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 213.PubMedCentralPubMed Kessler, K., & Rutherford, H. (2010). The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 213.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114(1), 72–88.CrossRefPubMed Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114(1), 72–88.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kessler, K., & Wang, H. (2012). Spatial perspective taking is an embodied process, but not for everyone in the same way: differences predicted by sex and social skills score. Spatial Cognition and Computation: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 133–158.CrossRef Kessler, K., & Wang, H. (2012). Spatial perspective taking is an embodied process, but not for everyone in the same way: differences predicted by sex and social skills score. Spatial Cognition and Computation: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 133–158.CrossRef
go back to reference Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability. Memory and Cognition, 29(5), 745–756.CrossRefPubMed Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability. Memory and Cognition, 29(5), 745–756.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lackner, J. R., & Graybiel, A. (1977). Somatosensory motion after-effect following earth-horizontal rotation about the Z-axis: a new illusion. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 48(6), 501–502. Lackner, J. R., & Graybiel, A. (1977). Somatosensory motion after-effect following earth-horizontal rotation about the Z-axis: a new illusion. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 48(6), 501–502.
go back to reference Lenggenhager, B., Lopez, C., & Blanke, O. (2008). Influence of galvanic vestibular stimulation on egocentric and object-based mental transformations. Experimental Brain Research, 184(2), 211–221.CrossRefPubMed Lenggenhager, B., Lopez, C., & Blanke, O. (2008). Influence of galvanic vestibular stimulation on egocentric and object-based mental transformations. Experimental Brain Research, 184(2), 211–221.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lopez, C., Bachofner, C., Mercier, M., & Blanke, O. (2009). Gravity and observer’s body orientation influence the visual perception of human body postures. Journal of Vision, 9(5), 1.1–14. Lopez, C., Bachofner, C., Mercier, M., & Blanke, O. (2009). Gravity and observer’s body orientation influence the visual perception of human body postures. Journal of Vision, 9(5), 1.1–14.
go back to reference Lopez, C., & Blanke, O. (2011). The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans. Brain Research Reviews, 67(1–2), 119–146.CrossRefPubMed Lopez, C., & Blanke, O. (2011). The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans. Brain Research Reviews, 67(1–2), 119–146.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lopez, C., Halje, P., & Blanke, O. (2008). Body ownership and embodiment: vestibular and multisensory mechanisms. Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(3), 149–161.CrossRefPubMed Lopez, C., Halje, P., & Blanke, O. (2008). Body ownership and embodiment: vestibular and multisensory mechanisms. Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(3), 149–161.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lopez, C., Heydrich, L., Seeck, M., & Blanke, O. (2010a). Abnormal self-location and vestibular vertigo in a patient with right frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 17(2), 289–292.CrossRefPubMed Lopez, C., Heydrich, L., Seeck, M., & Blanke, O. (2010a). Abnormal self-location and vestibular vertigo in a patient with right frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 17(2), 289–292.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lopez, C., Lenggenhager, B., & Blanke, O. (2010b). How vestibular stimulation interacts with illusory hand ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 33–47.CrossRefPubMed Lopez, C., Lenggenhager, B., & Blanke, O. (2010b). How vestibular stimulation interacts with illusory hand ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 33–47.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: transformation versus interference accounts. Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163–206.CrossRefPubMed May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: transformation versus interference accounts. Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163–206.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference May, M., & Wendt, M. (2012). Separating mental transformations and spatial compatibility effects in the own body transformation task. Cognitive Processing, 13(Suppl 1), S257–S260.CrossRefPubMed May, M., & Wendt, M. (2012). Separating mental transformations and spatial compatibility effects in the own body transformation task. Cognitive Processing, 13(Suppl 1), S257–S260.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mohr, C., Blanke, O., & Brugger, P. (2006). Perceptual aberrations impair mental own-body transformations. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(3), 528–534.CrossRefPubMed Mohr, C., Blanke, O., & Brugger, P. (2006). Perceptual aberrations impair mental own-body transformations. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(3), 528–534.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661–673.CrossRefPubMed Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661–673.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one’s body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(2), 172–191.CrossRef Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one’s body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(2), 172–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Petkova, V. I., Bjornsdotter, M., Gentile, G., Jonsson, T., Li, T. Q., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). From part- to whole-body ownership in the multisensory brain. Current Biology, 21(13), 1118–1122.CrossRefPubMed Petkova, V. I., Bjornsdotter, M., Gentile, G., Jonsson, T., Li, T. Q., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). From part- to whole-body ownership in the multisensory brain. Current Biology, 21(13), 1118–1122.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rieser, J. J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1157–1165.CrossRefPubMed Rieser, J. J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1157–1165.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rieser, J. J., Garing, A. E., & Young, M. F. (1994). Imagery, action, and young children’s spatial orientation: it’s not being there that counts, it’s what one has in mind. Child Development, 65(5), 1262–1278.CrossRefPubMed Rieser, J. J., Garing, A. E., & Young, M. F. (1994). Imagery, action, and young children’s spatial orientation: it’s not being there that counts, it’s what one has in mind. Child Development, 65(5), 1262–1278.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rieser, J. J., Guth, D. A., & Hill, E. W. (1986). Sensitivity to perspective structure while walking without vision. Perception, 15(2), 173–188.CrossRefPubMed Rieser, J. J., Guth, D. A., & Hill, E. W. (1986). Sensitivity to perspective structure while walking without vision. Perception, 15(2), 173–188.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shinder, M. E., & Taube, J. S. (2010). Differentiating ascending vestibular pathways to the cortex involved in spatial cognition. Journal of Vestibular Research, 20(1), 3–23.PubMed Shinder, M. E., & Taube, J. S. (2010). Differentiating ascending vestibular pathways to the cortex involved in spatial cognition. Journal of Vestibular Research, 20(1), 3–23.PubMed
go back to reference Sirigu, A., & Duhamel, J. R. (2001). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 910–919.CrossRefPubMed Sirigu, A., & Duhamel, J. R. (2001). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 910–919.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10(2), 199–204.CrossRefPubMed Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10(2), 199–204.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tadi, T., Overney, L. S., & Blanke, O. (2009). Three sequential brain activations encode mental transformations of upright and inverted human bodies: a high resolution evoked potential study. Neuroscience, 159(4), 1316–1325.CrossRefPubMed Tadi, T., Overney, L. S., & Blanke, O. (2009). Three sequential brain activations encode mental transformations of upright and inverted human bodies: a high resolution evoked potential study. Neuroscience, 159(4), 1316–1325.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110(1), 124–129.CrossRefPubMed Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110(1), 124–129.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Elk, M., & Blanke, O. (2012). Balancing bistable perception during self-motion. Experimental Brain Research, 222(3), 219–228. van Elk, M., & Blanke, O. (2012). Balancing bistable perception during self-motion. Experimental Brain Research, 222(3), 219–228.
go back to reference Viswanathan, S., Fritz, C., & Grafton, S. T. (2012). Telling the right hand from the left hand: multisensory integration, not motor imagery, solves the problem. Psychological Science, 23(6), 598–607.CrossRefPubMed Viswanathan, S., Fritz, C., & Grafton, S. T. (2012). Telling the right hand from the left hand: multisensory integration, not motor imagery, solves the problem. Psychological Science, 23(6), 598–607.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vogeley, K., May, M., Ritzl, A., Falkai, P., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2004). Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 817–827.CrossRefPubMed Vogeley, K., May, M., Ritzl, A., Falkai, P., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2004). Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 817–827.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wraga, M. (2003). Thinking outside the body: an advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 993–1005.CrossRefPubMed Wraga, M. (2003). Thinking outside the body: an advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 993–1005.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wraga, M., Shephard, J. M., Church, J. A., Inati, S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Imagined rotations of self versus objects: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1351–1361.CrossRefPubMed Wraga, M., Shephard, J. M., Church, J. A., Inati, S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Imagined rotations of self versus objects: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1351–1361.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zacks, J. M., & Michelon, P. (2005). Transformations of visuospatial images. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4(2), 96–118.CrossRefPubMed Zacks, J. M., & Michelon, P. (2005). Transformations of visuospatial images. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4(2), 96–118.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Imagined own-body transformations during passive self-motion
Auteurs
Michiel van Elk
Olaf Blanke
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0486-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2014

Psychological Research 1/2014 Naar de uitgave