Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2007

01-05-2007 | Original Article

Humans do not switch between path knowledge and landmarks when learning a new environment

Auteurs: Patrick Foo, Andrew Duchon, William H. Warren Jr., Michael J. Tarr

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Using a metric shortcut paradigm, we have found that like honeybees (Dyer in Animal Behaviour 41:239–246, 1991), humans do not seem to build a metric “cognitive map” from path integration. Instead, observers take novel shortcuts based on visual landmarks whenever they are available and reliable (Foo, Warren, Duchon, & Tarr in Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition 31(2):195–215, 2005). In the present experiment we examine whether humans, like ants (Wolf & Wehner in Journal of Experimental Biology 203:857–868, 2000), first use survey-type path knowledge, built up from path integration, and then subsequently shift to reliance on landmarks. In our study participants walked in an immersive virtual environment while head position and orientation were recorded. During training, participants learned two legs of a triangle with feedback: paths from Home to Red and Home to Blue. A configuration of colored posts surrounded the Red location. To test reliance on landmarks, these posts were covertly translated, rotated, or left unchanged during six probe trials. These probe trials were interspersed during the training procedure to measure changes over learning. Dependence on visual landmarks was immediate and sustained during training, and no significant learning effects were observed other than a decrease in hesitation time. Our results suggest that while humans have at least two distinct navigational strategies available to them, unlike ants, a computationally-simpler landmark strategy dominates during novel shortcut navigation.
Literatuur
go back to reference Aginsky, V. (2001). How visual landmarks are selected during small-scale navigation. Providence: Brown University. Aginsky, V. (2001). How visual landmarks are selected during small-scale navigation. Providence: Brown University.
go back to reference Appleyard, D. (1969). Why buildings are known: A predictive tool for architects and planners. Environment and Behavior, 1, 131–156.CrossRef Appleyard, D. (1969). Why buildings are known: A predictive tool for architects and planners. Environment and Behavior, 1, 131–156.CrossRef
go back to reference Appleyard, D. (1970). Styles and methods of structuring a city. Environment and Behavior, 2, 100–117.CrossRef Appleyard, D. (1970). Styles and methods of structuring a city. Environment and Behavior, 2, 100–117.CrossRef
go back to reference Batschelet, E. (1981). Circular statistics in biology. London: Academic Press. Batschelet, E. (1981). Circular statistics in biology. London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Bennett, A. T. D. (1996). Do animals have cognitive maps? Journal of Experimental Biology, 199(1), 219–224.PubMed Bennett, A. T. D. (1996). Do animals have cognitive maps? Journal of Experimental Biology, 199(1), 219–224.PubMed
go back to reference Bisch-Knaden, S., & Wehner, R. (2003a). Landmark memories are more robust when acquired at the nest site than en route: Experiments in desert ants. Naturwissenschaften, 90(3), 127–130.PubMed Bisch-Knaden, S., & Wehner, R. (2003a). Landmark memories are more robust when acquired at the nest site than en route: Experiments in desert ants. Naturwissenschaften, 90(3), 127–130.PubMed
go back to reference Bisch-Knaden, S., & Wehner, R. (2003b). Local vectors in desert ants: context-dependent landmark learning during outbound and homebound runs. Journal of Comparative Physiology. A. Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 189(3), 181–187. Bisch-Knaden, S., & Wehner, R. (2003b). Local vectors in desert ants: context-dependent landmark learning during outbound and homebound runs. Journal of Comparative Physiology. A. Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 189(3), 181–187.
go back to reference Chapuis, N. (1982). Mechanisms of spatial-behavior in a returning task in dogs. Annee Psychologique, 82(1), 75–100. Chapuis, N. (1982). Mechanisms of spatial-behavior in a returning task in dogs. Annee Psychologique, 82(1), 75–100.
go back to reference Chase, W. G. (1983). Spatial representation of taxi drivers. In D. Rogers, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skill (pp. 391–405). New York: Plenum. Chase, W. G. (1983). Spatial representation of taxi drivers. In D. Rogers, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skill (pp. 391–405). New York: Plenum.
go back to reference Ciancia, F. (1991). Tolman and Honzik (1930) revisited, or the mazes of psychology. The Psychological Record, 41, 461–472. Ciancia, F. (1991). Tolman and Honzik (1930) revisited, or the mazes of psychology. The Psychological Record, 41, 461–472.
go back to reference Collett, T. S., & Graham, P. (2004). Animal navigation: Path integration, visual landmarks and cognitive maps. Current Biology, 14(12), R475–R477.PubMedCrossRef Collett, T. S., & Graham, P. (2004). Animal navigation: Path integration, visual landmarks and cognitive maps. Current Biology, 14(12), R475–R477.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Collett, T. S., Collett, M., & Wehner, R. (2001). The guidance of desert ants by extended landmarks. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204(9), 1635–1639.PubMed Collett, T. S., Collett, M., & Wehner, R. (2001). The guidance of desert ants by extended landmarks. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204(9), 1635–1639.PubMed
go back to reference Dyer, F. C. (1991). Bees acquire route-based memories but not cognitive maps in a familiar landscape. Animal Behaviour, 41, 239–246.CrossRef Dyer, F. C. (1991). Bees acquire route-based memories but not cognitive maps in a familiar landscape. Animal Behaviour, 41, 239–246.CrossRef
go back to reference Dyer, F. C., Berry, N. A., & Richard, A. S. (1993). Honey bee spatial memory: use of route-based memories after displacement. Animal Behaviour, 45, 1028–1030.CrossRef Dyer, F. C., Berry, N. A., & Richard, A. S. (1993). Honey bee spatial memory: use of route-based memories after displacement. Animal Behaviour, 45, 1028–1030.CrossRef
go back to reference Eilam, D., Dank, M., & Maurer, R. (2003). Voles scale locomotion to the size of the open-field by adjusting the distance between stops: a possible link to path integration. Behavioural Brain Research, 141(1), 73–81.PubMedCrossRef Eilam, D., Dank, M., & Maurer, R. (2003). Voles scale locomotion to the size of the open-field by adjusting the distance between stops: a possible link to path integration. Behavioural Brain Research, 141(1), 73–81.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Etienne, A. S., Teroni, E., Hurni, C., & Portenier, V. (1990). The effect of a single light cue on homing behavior of the golden-hamster. Animal Behaviour, 39, 17–41.CrossRef Etienne, A. S., Teroni, E., Hurni, C., & Portenier, V. (1990). The effect of a single light cue on homing behavior of the golden-hamster. Animal Behaviour, 39, 17–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Etienne, A. S., Boulens, V., Maurer, R., Rowe, T., & Siegrist, C. (2000). A brief view of known landmarks reorientates path integration in hamsters. Naturwissenschaften, 87(11), 494–498.PubMedCrossRef Etienne, A. S., Boulens, V., Maurer, R., Rowe, T., & Siegrist, C. (2000). A brief view of known landmarks reorientates path integration in hamsters. Naturwissenschaften, 87(11), 494–498.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R., Boulens, V., Levy, A., & Rowe, T. (2004). Resetting the path integrator: a basic condition for route-based navigation. Journal Of Experimental Biology, 207(9), 1491–1508.PubMedCrossRef Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R., Boulens, V., Levy, A., & Rowe, T. (2004). Resetting the path integrator: a basic condition for route-based navigation. Journal Of Experimental Biology, 207(9), 1491–1508.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Foo, P., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A., & Tarr, M. J. (2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 31(2), 195–215.CrossRef Foo, P., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A., & Tarr, M. J. (2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 31(2), 195–215.CrossRef
go back to reference Foo, P., Harrison, M. C., Duchon, A. P., Warren, W. H. J., & Tarr, M. J. (2006). Humans follow perturbed landmarks during continuous path integration unless explicitly told to ignore them (in preparation). Foo, P., Harrison, M. C., Duchon, A. P., Warren, W. H. J., & Tarr, M. J. (2006). Humans follow perturbed landmarks during continuous path integration unless explicitly told to ignore them (in preparation).
go back to reference Fujita, N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (1990). A minimal representation for dead-reckoning navigation—updating the homing vector. Geographical Analysis, 22(4), 326–335. Fujita, N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (1990). A minimal representation for dead-reckoning navigation—updating the homing vector. Geographical Analysis, 22(4), 326–335.
go back to reference Fujita, N., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., & Golledge, R. G. (1993). The encoding-error model of pathway completion without vision. Geographical Analysis, 25(4), 295–314.CrossRef Fujita, N., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., & Golledge, R. G. (1993). The encoding-error model of pathway completion without vision. Geographical Analysis, 25(4), 295–314.CrossRef
go back to reference Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Gillner, S., & Mallot, H. A. (1998). Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge in a virtual maze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 445–463.PubMedCrossRef Gillner, S., & Mallot, H. A. (1998). Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge in a virtual maze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 445–463.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gould, J. L. (1986). The locale map of honey bees: Do insects have cognitive maps? Science, 232(4752), 861–863.CrossRefPubMed Gould, J. L. (1986). The locale map of honey bees: Do insects have cognitive maps? Science, 232(4752), 861–863.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gould J. L., & Gould, C. G. (1982). The insect mind: Physics or metaphysics? In D. R. Griffin (Ed.) Animal mind–human mind (pp. 269–298). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Gould J. L., & Gould, C. G. (1982). The insect mind: Physics or metaphysics? In D. R. Griffin (Ed.) Animal mind–human mind (pp. 269–298). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Hamilton, D. A., Driscoll, I., & Sutherland, R. J. (2002). Human place learning in a virtual Morris water task: Some important constraints on the flexibility of place navigation. Behavioral Brain Research, 1(2), 159–170.CrossRef Hamilton, D. A., Driscoll, I., & Sutherland, R. J. (2002). Human place learning in a virtual Morris water task: Some important constraints on the flexibility of place navigation. Behavioral Brain Research, 1(2), 159–170.CrossRef
go back to reference Kearns, M. J., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A. P., & Tarr, M. J. (2002). Path integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception, 31(3), 349–374.PubMedCrossRef Kearns, M. J., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A. P., & Tarr, M. J. (2002). Path integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception, 31(3), 349–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J., Golledge, R., Cicinelli, J. G., Doherty, S., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22(1), 19–43.PubMed Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J., Golledge, R., Cicinelli, J. G., Doherty, S., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22(1), 19–43.PubMed
go back to reference Landau, B., Spelke, E., & Gleitman, H. (1984). Spatial knowledge in a young blind child. Cognition, 16(3), 225–260.PubMedCrossRef Landau, B., Spelke, E., & Gleitman, H. (1984). Spatial knowledge in a young blind child. Cognition, 16(3), 225–260.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lee, T. (1962). “Brennan’s law” of shopping behaviour. Psychological Reports, 11, 662.CrossRef Lee, T. (1962). “Brennan’s law” of shopping behaviour. Psychological Reports, 11, 662.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, T. (1970). Urban neighborhood as a socio-spatial schema. Ekistics; An Introduction to the Science of Human Settlements, 177, 241–267. Lee, T. (1970). Urban neighborhood as a socio-spatial schema. Ekistics; An Introduction to the Science of Human Settlements, 177, 241–267.
go back to reference Lehrer, M., & Collett, T. S. (1994). Approaching and departing bees learn different cues to the distance of a landmark. Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 175, 171–177.CrossRef Lehrer, M., & Collett, T. S. (1994). Approaching and departing bees learn different cues to the distance of a landmark. Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 175, 171–177.CrossRef
go back to reference Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., Cicinelli, J. G., Pellegrino, J. W., & Fry, P. A. (1993). Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: Assessment of path integration ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 122(1), 73–91.PubMedCrossRef Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., Cicinelli, J. G., Pellegrino, J. W., & Fry, P. A. (1993). Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: Assessment of path integration ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 122(1), 73–91.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Maguire, E. A., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1997). Recalling routes around london: Activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(18), 7103–7110.PubMed Maguire, E. A., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1997). Recalling routes around london: Activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(18), 7103–7110.PubMed
go back to reference Maier, N. R. F. (1932). A study of orientation in the rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 14(3), 387–399.CrossRef Maier, N. R. F. (1932). A study of orientation in the rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 14(3), 387–399.CrossRef
go back to reference Menzel, R., Chittka, L., Eichmuller, S., Peitsch, D., & Knoll, P. (1990). Dominance of celestial cues over landmarks disproves map-like orientation in honey bees. Zeitschrift fur Naturforschung C, 45, 723–726. Menzel, R., Chittka, L., Eichmuller, S., Peitsch, D., & Knoll, P. (1990). Dominance of celestial cues over landmarks disproves map-like orientation in honey bees. Zeitschrift fur Naturforschung C, 45, 723–726.
go back to reference Morris, R. G., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N., & O'Keefe, J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297(5868), 681–683.PubMedCrossRef Morris, R. G., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N., & O'Keefe, J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297(5868), 681–683.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nadel, L. (1990). Varieties of spatial cognition—psychobiological considerations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 613–636.PubMedCrossRef Nadel, L. (1990). Varieties of spatial cognition—psychobiological considerations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 613–636.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference O'Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press. O'Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Péruch, P., Giraudo, M. D., & Garling, T. (1989). Distance cognition by taxi drivers and the general public. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 9(3), 233–239.CrossRef Péruch, P., Giraudo, M. D., & Garling, T. (1989). Distance cognition by taxi drivers and the general public. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 9(3), 233–239.CrossRef
go back to reference Péruch, P., May, M., & Wartenberg, F. (1997). Homing in virtual environments: Effects of field of view and path layout. Perception, 26(3), 301–311.PubMedCrossRef Péruch, P., May, M., & Wartenberg, F. (1997). Homing in virtual environments: Effects of field of view and path layout. Perception, 26(3), 301–311.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., van Veen, H., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2002). Visual homing is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in virtual reality. Presence-Teleoperators And Virtual Environments, 11(5), 443–473.CrossRef Riecke, B. E., van Veen, H., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2002). Visual homing is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in virtual reality. Presence-Teleoperators And Virtual Environments, 11(5), 443–473.CrossRef
go back to reference Spetch, M. L., Cheng, K., MacDonald, S. E., Linkenhoker, B. A., Kelly, D. M., & Doerkson, S. R. (1997). Use of landmark configuration in pigeons and humans.2. Generality across search tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(1), 14–24.CrossRef Spetch, M. L., Cheng, K., MacDonald, S. E., Linkenhoker, B. A., Kelly, D. M., & Doerkson, S. R. (1997). Use of landmark configuration in pigeons and humans.2. Generality across search tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(1), 14–24.CrossRef
go back to reference Suppes, P., Krantz, D. M., Luce, R. D., & Tversky, A. (1989). Foundations of measurement: Geometrical, threshold, and probabilistic representations (Vol. 2). San Diego: Academic Press, Inc. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Publishers. Suppes, P., Krantz, D. M., Luce, R. D., & Tversky, A. (1989). Foundations of measurement: Geometrical, threshold, and probabilistic representations (Vol. 2). San Diego: Academic Press, Inc. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Publishers.
go back to reference Tarr, M. J., & Warren, W. H. (2002). Virtual reality in behavioral neuroscience and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 5(Suppl), 1089–1092.PubMedCrossRef Tarr, M. J., & Warren, W. H. (2002). Virtual reality in behavioral neuroscience and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 5(Suppl), 1089–1092.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Teroni, E., Portenier, V., & Etienne, A. S. (1987). Spatial orientation of the golden-hamster in conditions of conflicting location-based and route-based information. Behavioral Ecology And Sociobiology, 20(6), 389–397.CrossRef Teroni, E., Portenier, V., & Etienne, A. S. (1987). Spatial orientation of the golden-hamster in conditions of conflicting location-based and route-based information. Behavioral Ecology And Sociobiology, 20(6), 389–397.CrossRef
go back to reference Thinus-Blanc, C. (1987). The cognitive map concept and its consequences. In P. Ellen, & C. Thinus-Blanc, (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal and man (pp. 1–19). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff, N.A.T.O A.S.I. series. Thinus-Blanc, C. (1987). The cognitive map concept and its consequences. In P. Ellen, & C. Thinus-Blanc, (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal and man (pp. 1–19). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff, N.A.T.O A.S.I. series.
go back to reference Thompson, W. B., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2004). Does the quality of the computer graphics matter when judging distances in virtually immersive environments? Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments, 13(5), 560–571.CrossRef Thompson, W. B., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2004). Does the quality of the computer graphics matter when judging distances in virtually immersive environments? Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments, 13(5), 560–571.CrossRef
go back to reference Tolman E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. The Psychological Review, 55(4), 189–208.CrossRef Tolman E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. The Psychological Review, 55(4), 189–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Trullier O., Wiener S. I., Berthoz A., & Meyer, J. A. (1997). Biologically-based artificial navigation systems: Review and prospects. Progress in Neurobiology, 51, 483–544.PubMedCrossRef Trullier O., Wiener S. I., Berthoz A., & Meyer, J. A. (1997). Biologically-based artificial navigation systems: Review and prospects. Progress in Neurobiology, 51, 483–544.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Waller, D., Loomis, J., Golledge, R., & Beall, A. C. (2000). Place learning in humans: The role of distance and direction information. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2, 333–354.CrossRef Waller, D., Loomis, J., Golledge, R., & Beall, A. C. (2000). Place learning in humans: The role of distance and direction information. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2, 333–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Human spatial representation: insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(9), 376–382.PubMedCrossRef Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Human spatial representation: insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(9), 376–382.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wehner, R., Bleuler, S., Nievergelt, C., & Shah, D. (1990). Bees navigate by using vectors and routes rather than maps. Naturwissenschaften, 77, 479–482.CrossRef Wehner, R., Bleuler, S., Nievergelt, C., & Shah, D. (1990). Bees navigate by using vectors and routes rather than maps. Naturwissenschaften, 77, 479–482.CrossRef
go back to reference Whishaw, I. Q., & Brooks, B. L. (1999). Calibrating space: Exploration is important for allothetic and idiothetic navigation. Hippocampus, 9(6), 659–667.PubMedCrossRef Whishaw, I. Q., & Brooks, B. L. (1999). Calibrating space: Exploration is important for allothetic and idiothetic navigation. Hippocampus, 9(6), 659–667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolf, H., & Wehner, R. (2000). Pinpointing food sources: Olfactory and anemotactic orientation in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 203, 857–868.PubMed Wolf, H., & Wehner, R. (2000). Pinpointing food sources: Olfactory and anemotactic orientation in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 203, 857–868.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Humans do not switch between path knowledge and landmarks when learning a new environment
Auteurs
Patrick Foo
Andrew Duchon
William H. Warren Jr.
Michael J. Tarr
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0080-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2007

Psychological Research 3/2007 Naar de uitgave