Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2006

01-03-2006 | Original Article

How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening?

Auteurs: Eric Ruthruff, Mark Van Selst, James C. Johnston, Roger Remington

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present study assessed three hypotheses of how practice reduces dual-task interference: Practice teaches participants to efficiently integrate performance of a task pair; practice promotes automatization of individual tasks, allowing the central bottleneck to be bypassed; practice leaves the bottleneck intact but shorter in duration. These hypotheses were tested in two transfer-of-training experiments. Participants received one of three training types (Task 1 only, or Task 2 only, or dual-task), followed by dual-task test sessions. Practice effects in Experiment 1 (Task 1: auditory–vocal; Task 2: visual–manual) were fully explained by the intact bottleneck hypothesis, without task integration or automatization. This hypothesis also accounted well for the majority of participants when the task order was reversed (Experiment 2). In this case, however, there were multiple indicators that several participants had succeeded in eliminating the bottleneck by automatizing one or both tasks. Neither experiment provided any evidence that practice promotes efficient task integration.
Voetnoten
1
Stage-shortening is an inevitable consequence of task practice. It plays a critical role in the intact bottleneck hypothesis—in fact, it is the sole cause of PRP reduction. Although the task integration and task automatization hypotheses also allow for stage-shortening, it is not assumed to be the principle cause of PRP reduction (e.g., if the bottleneck is bypassed, then interference will be small regardless of stage durations).
 
2
A chi-squared test revealed that this difference is statistically significant, χ 2 (1) = 5.581, p < .05. The difference between experiments is also significant using Fisher’s exact test (p = .0497, two-tailed), which is more appropriate given cell frequencies less than 5.
 
3
One complication for the hypothesis that Task 2 automatization was the key to bypassing the bottleneck is that one of the bypassers had not practiced Task 2 during the training phase. Nevertheless, this individual did respond very quickly to Task 2 at the long SOA (M = 376 ms) but not to Task 1 (M = 527 ms). It is possible that this person was naturally skilled at Task 2 and was able to achieve automatization relatively quickly (e.g., during the practice session).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, D. A., Antonis, B., & Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: A disproof of the single channel hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 225–235.PubMed Allport, D. A., Antonis, B., & Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: A disproof of the single channel hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 225–235.PubMed
go back to reference Bertelson, P., & Tisseyre, F. (1969). Refractory period of c-reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 122–128. Bertelson, P., & Tisseyre, F. (1969). Refractory period of c-reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 122–128.
go back to reference Davis, R. (1956). The limits of the “Psychological Refractory Period.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8, 24–38. Davis, R. (1956). The limits of the “Psychological Refractory Period.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8, 24–38.
go back to reference Davis, R. (1957). The human operator as a single channel information system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 119–129. Davis, R. (1957). The human operator as a single channel information system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 119–129.
go back to reference De Jong, R. (1993). Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 965–980.CrossRefPubMed De Jong, R. (1993). Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 965–980.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R. (1995). The role of preparation in overlapping-task performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 2–25.PubMed De Jong, R. (1995). The role of preparation in overlapping-task performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 2–25.PubMed
go back to reference Dutta, A., & Walker, B. N. (1995, November). Persistence of the PRP effect: Evaluating the response-selection bottleneck. Poster presented at the 36th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Los Angeles. Dutta, A., & Walker, B. N. (1995, November). Persistence of the PRP effect: Evaluating the response-selection bottleneck. Poster presented at the 36th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Los Angeles.
go back to reference Fletcher, B. C., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1978). The changing pattern of perceptual analytic strategies and response selection with practice in a two-choice reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 417–427.PubMed Fletcher, B. C., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1978). The changing pattern of perceptual analytic strategies and response selection with practice in a two-choice reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 417–427.PubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A., & Shulman, H. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 70–76.PubMed Greenwald, A., & Shulman, H. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 70–76.PubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once. I. Time-sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 52–57.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once. I. Time-sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 52–57.PubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (2003). On doing two things at once. III. Confirmation of perfect timesharing when simultaneous tasks are ideomotor compatible. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 859–868.CrossRefPubMed Greenwald, A. G. (2003). On doing two things at once. III. Confirmation of perfect timesharing when simultaneous tasks are ideomotor compatible. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 859–868.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Teague, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 527–545.CrossRefPubMed Hazeltine, E., Teague, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 527–545.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2004). The pairings of stimulus and responses modalities affect dual-task costs. Manuscript submitted for publication. Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2004). The pairings of stimulus and responses modalities affect dual-task costs. Manuscript submitted for publication.
go back to reference Hick, W. E. (1948). The discontinuous function of the human operator in pursuit tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 36–51. Hick, W. E. (1948). The discontinuous function of the human operator in pursuit tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 36–51.
go back to reference Hirst, W., Spelke, E. S., Reaves, C. C., Caharack, G., & Neisser, U. (1980). Dividing attention without alternation or automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 98–117. Hirst, W., Spelke, E. S., Reaves, C. C., Caharack, G., & Neisser, U. (1980). Dividing attention without alternation or automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 98–117.
go back to reference Johnston, J. C., & Delgado, D. F. (1993, November). Bypassing the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task performance. Paper presented to the 34th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Washington D.C. Johnston, J. C., & Delgado, D. F. (1993, November). Bypassing the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task performance. Paper presented to the 34th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Washington D.C.
go back to reference Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Karlin, L., & Kestenbaum, R. (1968). Effects of number of alternatives on the psychological refractory period. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 167–178.PubMed Karlin, L., & Kestenbaum, R. (1968). Effects of number of alternatives on the psychological refractory period. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 167–178.PubMed
go back to reference Keele, S. (1973). Attention and human performance. Pacific Palisades, CA: Goodyear. Keele, S. (1973). Attention and human performance. Pacific Palisades, CA: Goodyear.
go back to reference Levy, J., & Pashler, H. (2001). Is dual-task slowing instruction dependent? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 862–869.CrossRefPubMed Levy, J., & Pashler, H. (2001). Is dual-task slowing instruction dependent? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 862–869.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238. Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., & Ruthruff, E. (2004). Task switching in a hierarchical task structure: Evidence for the fragility of the task repetition benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 697–713. Lien, M.-C., & Ruthruff, E. (2004). Task switching in a hierarchical task structure: Evidence for the fragility of the task repetition benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 697–713.
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., Proctor, R. W., & Allen, P. A. (2002). Ideomotor compatibility in the psychological refractory period effect: 29 years of oversimplification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 396–409.CrossRefPubMed Lien, M.-C., Proctor, R. W., & Allen, P. A. (2002). Ideomotor compatibility in the psychological refractory period effect: 29 years of oversimplification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 396–409.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., Proctor, R. W., & Ruthruff, E. (2003). Still no evidence for perfect timesharing with two ideomotor compatible tasks: An observation on Greenwald (2003). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1267–1272.CrossRefPubMed Lien, M.-C., Proctor, R. W., & Ruthruff, E. (2003). Still no evidence for perfect timesharing with two ideomotor compatible tasks: An observation on Greenwald (2003). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1267–1272.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., McCann, R. E., Ruthruff, E., & Proctor, R. W. (in press). Processing limitations in dual-task performance: Can the central bottleneck be bypassed with ideomotor compatible tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Lien, M.-C., McCann, R. E., Ruthruff, E., & Proctor, R. W. (in press). Processing limitations in dual-task performance: Can the central bottleneck be bypassed with ideomotor compatible tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
go back to reference Maquestiaux, F., Hartley, A. A., Bertsch, J. (in press). Can practice overcome age-related differences in the psychological refractory period effect? Psychology and Aging. Maquestiaux, F., Hartley, A. A., Bertsch, J. (in press). Can practice overcome age-related differences in the psychological refractory period effect? Psychology and Aging.
go back to reference McCann, R. S., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 471–484.CrossRef McCann, R. S., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 471–484.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997a). A computational theory of human multiple-task performance: The EPIC information-processing architecture and strategic response deferment model. Psychological Review, 104, 1–65. Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997a). A computational theory of human multiple-task performance: The EPIC information-processing architecture and strategic response deferment model. Psychological Review, 104, 1–65.
go back to reference Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997b). A computational theory of human multiple-task performance. II. Accounts of psychological refractory phenomena. Psychological Review, 107, 749–791. Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997b). A computational theory of human multiple-task performance. II. Accounts of psychological refractory phenomena. Psychological Review, 107, 749–791.
go back to reference Mowbray, G. H., & Rhoades, M. V. (1959). On the reduction of choice reaction times with practice. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36. Mowbray, G. H., & Rhoades, M. V. (1959). On the reduction of choice reaction times with practice. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 330–342.CrossRefPubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 330–342.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning. I. Locus of practice effects in speeded choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 20–32.CrossRef Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning. I. Locus of practice effects in speeded choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 20–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45. Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1998). Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 155–189). Hove, UK: Psychology. Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1998). Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 155–189). Hove, UK: Psychology.
go back to reference Pashler, H., Carrier, M., & Hoffman, J. (1993). Saccadic eye movements and dual-task interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 51–82. Pashler, H., Carrier, M., & Hoffman, J. (1993). Saccadic eye movements and dual-task interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 51–82.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Nissen, J. M., & Klein, R. (1976). Visual dominance: An information processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83, 157–171.CrossRefPubMed Posner, M. I., Nissen, J. M., & Klein, R. (1976). Visual dominance: An information processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83, 157–171.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Miller, J. O., & Lachmann, T. (1995). Does mental rotation require central mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 552–570.CrossRefPubMed Ruthruff, E., Miller, J. O., & Lachmann, T. (1995). Does mental rotation require central mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 552–570.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H. E., & Klaassen, A. (2001). Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: Structural limitation or voluntary postponement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 73–80. Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H. E., & Klaassen, A. (2001). Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: Structural limitation or voluntary postponement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 73–80.
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Johnston, J. C., & Van Selst, M. (2001). Why practice reduces dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 3–21.CrossRefPubMed Ruthruff, E., Johnston, J. C., & Van Selst, M. (2001). Why practice reduces dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 3–21.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H., & Hazeltine, E. (2003). Dual-task interference with equal task emphasis: Graded capacity-sharing or central postponement? Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 801–816.PubMed Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H., & Hazeltine, E. (2003). Dual-task interference with equal task emphasis: Graded capacity-sharing or central postponement? Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 801–816.PubMed
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Johnston, J. C., Van Selst, M. A., Whitsell, S., & Remington, R. (2003). Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: Has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 280–289.CrossRefPubMed Ruthruff, E., Johnston, J. C., Van Selst, M. A., Whitsell, S., & Remington, R. (2003). Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: Has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 280–289.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D., Lauber, E., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: Uncorking the central attentional bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101–108.CrossRefPubMed Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D., Lauber, E., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: Uncorking the central attentional bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101–108.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schweickert, R. (1978). A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a Stroop task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 18, 105–139.CrossRef Schweickert, R. (1978). A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a Stroop task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 18, 105–139.CrossRef
go back to reference Shaffer, L. H. (1975). Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.) Attention and Performance V (pp.157–167). San Diego, CA: Academic. Shaffer, L. H. (1975). Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.) Attention and Performance V (pp.157–167). San Diego, CA: Academic.
go back to reference Spelke, E. S., Hirst, W. C., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–230.CrossRef Spelke, E. S., Hirst, W. C., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2000, November). Is the PRP effect due to a strategic or structural bottleneck? Paper presented at the 41st annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA. Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2000, November). Is the PRP effect due to a strategic or structural bottleneck? Paper presented at the 41st annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA.
go back to reference Van Selst, M., & Johnston, J. C. (1997, November). Modality modulates PRP interference. Presented at the 38th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Philadelphia. Van Selst, M., & Johnston, J. C. (1997, November). Modality modulates PRP interference. Presented at the 38th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Philadelphia.
go back to reference Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 631–650. Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 631–650.
go back to reference Van Selst, M., Ruthruff, E., & Johnston, J. C. (1999). Can practice eliminate the Psychological Refractory Period effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1268–1283.CrossRefPubMed Van Selst, M., Ruthruff, E., & Johnston, J. C. (1999). Can practice eliminate the Psychological Refractory Period effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1268–1283.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19. Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1976). Skilled performance: Perceptual and motor skills. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Welford, A. T. (1976). Skilled performance: Perceptual and motor skills. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.
go back to reference Wickens, C. D., & Liu, Y. (1988). Codes and modalities in multiple resources: A success and a qualification. Human Factors, 30, 599–616.PubMed Wickens, C. D., & Liu, Y. (1988). Codes and modalities in multiple resources: A success and a qualification. Human Factors, 30, 599–616.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening?
Auteurs
Eric Ruthruff
Mark Van Selst
James C. Johnston
Roger Remington
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0192-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2006

Psychological Research 2/2006 Naar de uitgave