Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2019

17-10-2017 | Original Article

How conceptual overlap and modality pairings affect task-switching and mixing costs

Auteurs: Jonathan Schacherer, Eliot Hazeltine

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Manipulating the pairings of stimulus and response modalities has been shown to affect how response selection processes for distinct tasks interact. For example, Stephan and Koch (Psychol Res 75(6):491–498, 2011) found smaller performance costs when participants switched between visual–manual (VM) and auditory–vocal (AV) tasks (modality compatible; MC) compared to between visual–vocal (VV) and auditory–manual (AM) tasks (modality incompatible; MI). However, in the Stephan and Koch study, there was conceptual overlap between one set of stimuli and one set of responses. For the MC pair, these stimuli and responses belonged to the same task, whereas for the MI pair, they belonged to different tasks. To examine how conceptual overlap affected switch and mixing costs, we conducted two experiments. Experiment 1a was a near replication of Stephan and Koch in which conceptual overlap was present in the MC AV task. In contrast, Experiment 1b reduced conceptual overlap within the MC AV task and increased it in the MI VV task. In Experiment 1a, we replicated Stephan and Koch’s findings: larger switch costs were observed for the MI pair; in Experiment 1b, we found numerically greater switch costs in the MC condition. In Experiment 2, we reduced conceptual overlap in both tasks and found no effect of modality compatibility on switch costs. However, mixing costs were primarily driven by modality compatibility, regardless of conceptual overlap. These results highlight the different roles that conceptual overlap and modality pairings have on switch and mixing costs.
Voetnoten
1
We defined ‘equipment failure’ as vocal response accuracy less than 0.70. This was caused by our speech recognition program inaccurately identifying vocal responses. Of the subjects we discarded, average accuracy on vocal trials was: Exp. 1a: 0.48; Exp. 1b: 0.55; Exp. 2: 0.56. We set a criterion of 0.70 and subjects whose data did not meet that criterion were not analyzed further.
 
2
Mean RT in Experiment 1 did not significantly differ between the two possible S–R mappings containing conceptual overlap, (e.g., M → “A”, X → “one” vs. M → “one”, X → “A”), Exp. 1a: t(14) = 0.947, p = 0.360; Exp. 1b: t(14) = 1.795, p = 0.094]. The absence of a significant effect is consistent with our proposal that the conceptual overlap does not rely on dimensional overlap leading to individual stimuli differentially activating specific responses, as when there is element level compatibility (e.g., Fitts & Seeger, 1953; Kornblum et al., 1990).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, A., Styles, E., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press. Allport, A., Styles, E., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (1999). Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. M. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space and action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 273–296). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (1999). Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. M. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space and action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 273–296). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Fintor, E., Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2017). Emerging features of modality mappings in task switching: Modality compatibility requires variability at the level of both stimulus and response modality. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-017-0875-5.CrossRefPubMed Fintor, E., Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2017). Emerging features of modality mappings in task switching: Modality compatibility requires variability at the level of both stimulus and response modality. Psychological Research. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-017-0875-5.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fitts, P. M., & Deininger, R. L. (1954). S-R compatibility—correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(6), 483–492. doi:10.1037/h0054967.CrossRefPubMed Fitts, P. M., & Deininger, R. L. (1954). S-R compatibility—correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(6), 483–492. doi:10.​1037/​h0054967.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A choice reaction time test of ideomotor theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(1), 20–25.CrossRefPubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A choice reaction time test of ideomotor theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(1), 20–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., & Shulman, H. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101(1), 70–76.CrossRefPubMed Greenwald, A. G., & Shulman, H. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101(1), 70–76.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5–82. Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5–82.
go back to reference Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874. doi:10.1037/a0019842.CrossRefPubMed Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874. doi:10.​1037/​a0019842.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S. (1992). Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus compatibility. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior II (pp. 743–777). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Kornblum, S. (1992). Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus compatibility. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior II (pp. 743–777). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.CrossRefPubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855–875. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.4.855.CrossRefPubMed Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855–875. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​21.​4.​855.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Influence of irrelevant information on human performance: Effects of S–R association strength and relative timing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(1), 95–136. doi:10.1080/02724980042000048.CrossRefPubMed Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Influence of irrelevant information on human performance: Effects of S–R association strength and relative timing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(1), 95–136. doi:10.​1080/​0272498004200004​8.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 435–448.PubMed Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 435–448.PubMed
go back to reference Philipp, A. M., Weidner, R., Koch, I., & Fink, G. R. (2013). Differential roles of inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex in task switching: Evidence from stimulus-categorization switching and response-modality switching. Human Brain Mapping, 34(8), 1910–1920. doi:10.1002/hbm.22036.CrossRefPubMed Philipp, A. M., Weidner, R., Koch, I., & Fink, G. R. (2013). Differential roles of inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex in task switching: Evidence from stimulus-categorization switching and response-modality switching. Human Brain Mapping, 34(8), 1910–1920. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​22036.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Wang, H. F., & Vu, K. P. L. (2002). Influences of different combinations of conceptual, perceptual, and structural similarity on stimulus–response compatibility. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(1), 59–74.CrossRefPubMed Proctor, R. W., Wang, H. F., & Vu, K. P. L. (2002). Influences of different combinations of conceptual, perceptual, and structural similarity on stimulus–response compatibility. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(1), 59–74.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.PubMed Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.PubMed
go back to reference Stelzel, C., Schumacher, E. H., Schubert, T., & D’Esposito, M. (2006). The neural effect of stimulus–response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: An fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70(6), 514–525. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0013-7.CrossRefPubMed Stelzel, C., Schumacher, E. H., Schubert, T., & D’Esposito, M. (2006). The neural effect of stimulus–response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: An fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70(6), 514–525. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-005-0013-7.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input–output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081. doi:10.1037/a0019695.CrossRefPubMed Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input–output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081. doi:10.​1037/​a0019695.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2016). Modality-specific effects on crosstalk in task switching: Evidence from modality compatibility using bimodal stimulation. Psychological Research, 80(6), 935–943.CrossRefPubMed Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2016). Modality-specific effects on crosstalk in task switching: Evidence from modality compatibility using bimodal stimulation. Psychological Research, 80(6), 935–943.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wickens, C. D., Sandry, D. L., & Vidulich, M. (1983). Compatibility and resource competition between modalities of input, central processing, and output. Human Factors, 25(2), 227–248.CrossRefPubMed Wickens, C. D., Sandry, D. L., & Vidulich, M. (1983). Compatibility and resource competition between modalities of input, central processing, and output. Human Factors, 25(2), 227–248.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
How conceptual overlap and modality pairings affect task-switching and mixing costs
Auteurs
Jonathan Schacherer
Eliot Hazeltine
Publicatiedatum
17-10-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0932-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2019

Psychological Research 5/2019 Naar de uitgave