Choice repetition rate (CRR)
A 2 (Goal Congruency
N−1: incongruent, congruent) × 2 (Reliability: reliable, unreliable) × 2 (Order: reliable-unreliable, unreliable-reliable) mixed-factors ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Goal Congruency
N−1,
F (1,58) = 11.85,
p = .001, η
p2 = 0.17, BF
10 = 29.183: The CRR following incongruent trials (
M = 49%,
SD = 16) was higher than the CRR following congruent trials (
M = 45%,
SD = 15). Furthermore, the interaction between Goal Congruency
N−1 and Reliability was significant,
F (1,58) = 7.23,
p = .009, η
p2 = 0.11, BF
10 = 5.962: The difference between the CRR following incongruent trials (
M = 51%,
SD = 18) and the CRR following congruent trials (
M = 44%,
SD = 15) for participants collaborating with a reliable co-actor was larger than the difference of the CRR following incongruent trials (
M = 48%,
SD = 17) and the CRR following congruent trials (
M = 46%,
SD = 16) for participants collaborating with an unreliable co-actor (see Fig.
2, Panel A). All other effects were not significant,
Fs ≤ 1.08,
ps ≥ 0.304. Subsequent t-tests showed a significant effect of GoalCongruency
N−1 for participants cooperating with a reliable co-actor,
t(59) = 4.05,
p < .001,
dz = 0.52, BF
10 = 150.065, but not for those cooperating with an unreliable co-actor,
t(59) = 1.30,
p = .198,
dz = 0.17, BF
10 = 0.315. Like in Experiment 1, these results show that participants were again more likely to repeat their choice from the previous trial after incongruent trials. Moreover, this choice repetition effect was further modulated by the reliability of the respective co-actor: Participants showed the goal repetition effect in the test block with the reliable co-actor, but not in the test block with the other, unreliable co-actor.
Table 2
Mean choice repetition rate (SD) of experiment 3 as percentage
CRR (%) | 49 (16) | 45 (15) | | | | |
reliable first | | | 51 (20) | 45 (18) | 49 (18) | 46 (16) |
unreliable first | | | 52 (15) | 43 (13) | 47 (16) | 46 (17) |
General discussion
Sociomotor theory applies ideomotor ideas to social contexts when it suggests that one’s actions can also be represented in terms of the effects they elicit from others, resulting in anticipation of
social action effects (Kunde et al.,
2018). Applying sociomotor theory to a joint action setting (Sebanz et al.,
2006), we investigated individual’s goal selection when a co-actor’s (a confederate) actions had violated the former’s action-effect anticipation. In addition, we manipulated how often the co-actor violated the participant’s action-effect anticipation per block, thus creating contexts varying in predictability (and/or contingency; cf. Elsner & Hommel,
2004). In a novel joint goal-setting paradigm, the co-actor first always continued the participant’s target movement (100% congruent trials). In the following test block(s), the co-actor’s compliance with the participants’ directional choices varied (50% vs. 80% of congruent trials). In Experiment 1 (consisting of one test block), participants repeated their corner choices more often after incongruent trials. Critically, this goal repetition effect was only present for participants collaborating with a (mostly) reliable co-actor (80% congruent trials), but not for those collaborating with an unreliable co-actor (50% congruent trials; between-participants design). In Experiment 2, participants collaborated with a co-actor who changed his reliability between two test blocks (order counterbalanced across participants). Here, the results of Experiment 1 could not be replicated. Experiment 3 was almost identical to Experiment 2, but – to make the change in reliability more salient - the co-actor changed in addition to his/her reliability in-between test blocks. As a result, we again found a higher choice repetition rate after incongruent trials, but only when interacting with a reliable co-actor. Subjective ratings of participants’ feeling of control regarding the movements of the target indicated that participants felt most in control in the learning block and were able to differentiate between a reliable and an unreliable co-actor across all three experiments.
The study presented here showed, for the first time, how action effects produced by a co-actor can systematically alter action selection in a novel joint action paradigm. Here, participants in an acquisition phase first learned action effect associations between their own action (left or right corner choice) and the spatially congruent effect produced by the co-actor (who always moved the star to the congruent corner). In the then following test phase, these action effect-anticipations were occasionally violated (when the co-actor moved the star to the incongruent corner). Results show that participants choice was repeated more often after expectation violations, but only when interacting with a reliable co-actor. The results allow for three different interpretations that may not be mutually exclusive. (1) In terms of sociomotor theory (Kunde et al.,
2018), repeating one’s unrealized directional choice from the previous trial might have been the participant’s means of producing compliant behavior from the co-actor (like in a leader-follower dyad). That is, from this perspective, participant’s choice repetition in response to violations of expectation (after incongruent trials) were driven by the intention to change the co-actors behavior (see also Pesquita et al.,
2018; Vesper et al.,
2010). Alternatively (2) and in line with ideomotor-theory, the participant’s primary goal was not to change the behavior of the co-actor. Instead participants’ action selection was continuously driven by their anticipated (and occasionally formerly unfulfilled) action-effect (cf. Prinz,
1997). From this perspective, insisting on co-actor compliance might be seen as the participants’ means of trying to produce the anticipated but unrealized target movement from the previous trial. Thus, given the social context used in the paradigm, choice repetition could be either a non-social attempt at delayed goal realization or, in terms of the minimal architecture of joint action (Vesper et al.,
2010), an attempt at nonverbal communication to the co-actor and/or follower (referred to there as a ‘coordination smoother’; see also Heintz & Scott-Phillips,
2022; Vesper & Richardson,
2014). Last, but not least (3), the results may also be interpreted as an indication of the Zeigarnik-effect (Goschke & Kuhl,
1993; Zeigarnik,
1927), according to which an unfulfilled action rests in a state of heightened activation and might therefore be repeated on the next occasion (cf. Fröber & Dreisbach,
2023). Note that the latter two interpretations may be independent from the presence of a social partner. However, because the effect presented here did not survive a simple change in reliability from one block to the next (Experiment 2), we are tempted to assume that the social context had an impact, be it only to make the change in reliability (or predictability) of the environment more salient. Note that it is difficult to disentangle the role of mechanisms based on actor reliability/predictability or goal completion motivations, because the co-actor’s target movement and the final target location are hardly separable in the context of the novel paradigm.
2
The question whether the goal repetition effect demonstrated in the present research is truly social in nature also tackles current discussions about the potential non-social nature of sociomotor action control (e.g., Neszmélyi et al.,
2022; Weller et al.,
2019; see also Kim & Hommel,
2019). The present findings may well be replicable in a nonsocial setting, especially if participants’ choice repetition behavior is primarily directed at the outcome rather than the actions of the co-actor. For example, sociomotor studies that similarly inserted another person’s contribution into the chain from a participant’s action to its effect found no differences in sensory processing (Neszmélyi & Horváth,
2021) or performance (Müller,
2016) compared to analogous non-social setups. Moreover, since human actions are driven by the pursuit of goals
3 (e.g., Heckhausen & Heckhausen,
2018; Hommel,
2022; McClelland,
1988), the goal repetition effect might well be interpreted in terms of non-social goal persistence (Feather,
1962; Moshontz & Hoyle,
2021; for a recent review, see Brandstätter & Bernecker,
2022). Regarding a potential role of co-representation, there is a large body of research showing that participants’ beliefs about their (non-human) partner’s intentionality are crucial for joint action effects (e.g., Ruys & Aarts,
2010; Sahaï et al.,
2019; Tsai et al.,
2008; for a recent review, see Miss et al.,
2022). Thus, to address the social nature of the present research findings, a more promising avenue for future research might be to manipulate social characteristics of the co-actor, such as his/her friendliness or competence, which have been shown to modulate the degree of co-representation (e.g., Ruys & Aarts,
2010; Tufft,
2022) and are difficult to reconcile with non-social accounts (e.g., referential response coding; Dolk et al.,
2013).
The present research contributes to ideomotor-inspired research in social settings in several ways. Methodologically, sociomotor studies had typically used a leader-follower dyad in which the participant initiates the ‘interaction’ and a co-actor responds with predetermined responses, usually by imitating or counter-imitating the participant’s actions (e.g., Pfister et al.,
2013). In the present research, the actions of the participant and the co-actor were never identical, and the actions of the former only indirectly targeted the responses of the latter. Moreover, in the practice and test blocks, the participants were led to believe that the co-actor was also free to decide to which end position he wanted to move the target. So far, only few studies on sociomotor action control involved such higher level goal pursuit and (mutual) coordination between co-actors (e.g., Müller,
2020; for a recent review, see Neszmélyi et al.,
2022). Regarding the dependent variable, ideomotor studies typically measure performance benefits for compatible (e.g., Kunde,
2001) or learned (e.g., Elsner & Hommel,
2001) action-effect associations in terms of response times and error rates. By contrast, in the present research, the goal repetition effect describes a systematic influence on participants’ action selection.
Naturally, the present research has limitations. For instance, participants in Experiment 2 indicated in their subjective control ratings that they understood the reliability difference of the co-actor’s target movements between one test block (80% congruent trials) and the other (50% congruent trials). The critical question is why they did not adapt their choice behavior accordingly. From a cognitive perspective, the change in reliability may not have been perceived early enough to influence goal choice (e.g., Bouton,
2010), but rather in hindsight when feelings of control were assessed. Changing the co-actor along with the reliability might have made this change more salient. From a social perspective, one might argue that participants collaborating with a co-actor that turned from unreliable to reliable did not trust his/her apparent change of mind. Instead, they might have concluded from his/her unreliability in the first test block that s/he is generally unreliable (cf. Jones & Davis,
1965). In addition, participants and experimenters were not systematically paired with respect to sex/gender, even though previous research has shown weaker joint action effects in mixed- than in same-sex dyads (Fabbri et al.,
2023).
To conclude, the here introduced novel (joint) action selection paradigm has proven to be well suited to study sociomotor action effects (Kunde et al.,
2018, for a recent review, see Neszmélyi et al.,
2022). We have provided first evidence that if a co-actor violates one’s action effect anticipation individuals tend to repeat their goal selection, presumably in order to see their internal goals realized (cf. Prinz,
1997). This goal repetition effect, however, is critically dependent on the overall reliability of the co-actor.