Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

10-08-2020 | Original Article

Heterogenous abstract concepts: is “ponder” different from “dissolve”?

Auteurs: Emiko J. Muraki, David M. Sidhu, Penny M. Pexman

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2022

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Abstract words have usually been treated as a homogenous group, with limited investigation of the influence of different underlying representational systems for these words. In the present study we examined lexical–semantic processing of abstract verbs, separating them into mental state, emotional state and nonembodied state types. We used a syntactic classification task and a memory task to investigate behavioural differences amongst the abstract verb types. Semantic richness effects of each of the verbs' associates were then investigated to determine the relationship of linguistic associations to semantic processing response times for abstract verbs. We found a modest effect of abstract verb type, with mental state abstract verbs processed more quickly than nonembodied abstract verbs in the syntactic classification task; however, this effect was task dependent. We also found that memory was less accurate for the mental state abstract verbs. The semantic richness analysis of abstract verb associates revealed (1) that the concreteness of an abstract verb’s associates has a positive relationship to the verb’s response time and (2) a negative relationship between response time and age of acquisition for associates of nonembodied verbs. The results provide support for the proposal that abstract concepts engage complex representations in modal and linguistic systems.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The response time data from Experiments 1–3 exhibited a positive skew. Supplementary analyses using a log-transformed response time for all mixed effect models reported here found that all significant fixed effects remained when using the log-transformed response time, with the exception of the significant difference between nonembodied abstract verbs and embodied verbs reported for Experiment 2. This effect was also not present in Experiment 1, which likely indicates this is not a strong or reliable effect as discussed in the discussion section.
 
2
Mixed effect models were also conducted using the response data for the Experiment 2 encoding task and the SCT from Experiment 3. Those results are consistent with the AoA and concreteness effects reported here. The frequency of associates effect from Experiment 1 was not replicated in the data from Experiment 2 or 3.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Altarriba, J., & Bauer, L. M. (2004). The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: A comparison between emotion, abstract and concrete words. The American Journal of Psychology, 117(3), 389–410.CrossRefPubMed Altarriba, J., & Bauer, L. M. (2004). The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: A comparison between emotion, abstract and concrete words. The American Journal of Psychology, 117(3), 389–410.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (1987). The instability of graded structure in concepts. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101–140). New York: Cambridge University Press. Barsalou, L. W. (1987). The instability of graded structure in concepts. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101–140). New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245–283). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245–283). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., & Weimer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory (pp. 129–163). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W., & Weimer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory (pp. 129–163). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bird, H., Franklin, S., & Howard, D. (2001). Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words. Behavior Research Methods, 33(1), 73–70. Bird, H., Franklin, S., & Howard, D. (2001). Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words. Behavior Research Methods, 33(1), 73–70.
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words as social tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. Berlin and New York: Springer.CrossRef Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words as social tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. Berlin and New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bolte, J., & Bohl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424.CrossRefPubMed Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bolte, J., & Bohl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911.CrossRefPubMed Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C. (2001). Characterizing semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychological Bulletin, 8(3), 531–544.CrossRef Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C. (2001). Characterizing semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychological Bulletin, 8(3), 531–544.CrossRef
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M. (2015). Few believe the world is flat: How embodiment is changing the scientific understanding of cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 165–171.CrossRefPubMed Glenberg, A. M. (2015). Few believe the world is flat: How embodiment is changing the scientific understanding of cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 165–171.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermuller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.CrossRefPubMed Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermuller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(2), 130–136. James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(2), 130–136.
go back to reference Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 804–823.PubMed Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 804–823.PubMed
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The Embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The Embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Locker, L., Simpson, G. B., & Yates, M. (2003). Semantic neighborhood effects on the recognition of ambiguous words. Memory and Cognition, 31(4), 505–515.CrossRefPubMed Locker, L., Simpson, G. B., & Yates, M. (2003). Semantic neighborhood effects on the recognition of ambiguous words. Memory and Cognition, 31(4), 505–515.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McKoon, G., & Macfarland, T. (2002). Event templates in the lexical representations of verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 1–44.CrossRefPubMed McKoon, G., & Macfarland, T. (2002). Event templates in the lexical representations of verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 1–44.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Perea, M., Rosa, E., & Gomez, C. (2002). Is the go/no-go lexical decision task an alternative to the yes/no lexical decision task? Memory and Cognition, 30(1), 34–45.CrossRefPubMed Perea, M., Rosa, E., & Gomez, C. (2002). Is the go/no-go lexical decision task an alternative to the yes/no lexical decision task? Memory and Cognition, 30(1), 34–45.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pexman, P. M. (2012). Meaning-based influences on visual word recognition. In J. S. Adelman (Ed.), Visual word recognition meaning and context, individuals, and development (2nd ed., pp. 24–43). Hove: Psychology Press. Pexman, P. M. (2012). Meaning-based influences on visual word recognition. In J. S. Adelman (Ed.), Visual word recognition meaning and context, individuals, and development (2nd ed., pp. 24–43). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Pexman, P. M., Hargreaves, I. S., Edwards, J. D., Henry, L. C., & Goodyear, B. G. (2007). Neural correlates of concreteness in semantic categorization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1407–1419.CrossRefPubMed Pexman, P. M., Hargreaves, I. S., Edwards, J. D., Henry, L. C., & Goodyear, B. G. (2007). Neural correlates of concreteness in semantic categorization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1407–1419.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pylyshyn, Z. (1980). Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundations of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 111–169.CrossRef Pylyshyn, Z. (1980). Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundations of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 111–169.CrossRef
go back to reference Winter, B. (2020). Statistics for linguists: An introduction using R. New York, NY: Routledge. Winter, B. (2020). Statistics for linguists: An introduction using R. New York, NY: Routledge.
Metagegevens
Titel
Heterogenous abstract concepts: is “ponder” different from “dissolve”?
Auteurs
Emiko J. Muraki
David M. Sidhu
Penny M. Pexman
Publicatiedatum
10-08-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2022
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01398-x