Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 5/2013

01-05-2013 | Original Paper

From Action to Interaction: Exploring the Contribution of Body Motion Cues to Social Understanding in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Auteurs: Laurie Centelles, Christine Assaiante, Katallin Etchegoyhen, Manuel Bouvard, Christina Schmitz

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 5/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Two studies investigated whether typically developing children (TD) and children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) were able to decide whether two characters were communicating or not on the basis of point-light displays. Point-lights portrayed actors engaged or not in a social interaction. In study 1, TD children (4–10 years old; n = 36) grasped social intentions from body language, with a notable improvement around 7/8. In study 2, children with ASD (6–12 years old; n = 12) could categorize the point-light displays at above-chance levels, but performed less efficiently, especially for the social interaction displays, than TD children (matched to chronological and non-verbal mental age, 6–12 years old; n = 24). An action representation deficit is discussed in relation to a social representation deficit and it is suggested that these deficits might be linked to altered maturational process of the mirror system in ASD.
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), 4th edn. Washington, DC. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), 4th edn. Washington, DC.
go back to reference Asendorpf, J. B., & Baudonniere, P. M. (1993). Self-awareness and other-awareness—mirror self-recognition and synchronic imitation among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 29, 88–95.CrossRef Asendorpf, J. B., & Baudonniere, P. M. (1993). Self-awareness and other-awareness—mirror self-recognition and synchronic imitation among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 29, 88–95.CrossRef
go back to reference Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3023–3029.PubMedCrossRef Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3023–3029.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746.PubMedCrossRef Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Webb, S., & Murias, M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 64, 228–237.PubMedCrossRef Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Webb, S., & Murias, M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 64, 228–237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Kramer, S. J. (1987). Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: Implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 577–585.PubMedCrossRef Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Kramer, S. J. (1987). Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: Implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 577–585.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex’’ issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225.PubMedCrossRef Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex’’ issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.PubMedCrossRef Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.PubMedCrossRef Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedCrossRef Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Centelles, L., Assaiante, C., Nazarian, B., Anton, J. L., & Schmitz, C. (2011). Recruitment of both the mirror and the mentalizing networks when observing social interactions depicted by point-lights: A neuroimaging study. PLoS ONE, 6(1), e15749.PubMedCrossRef Centelles, L., Assaiante, C., Nazarian, B., Anton, J. L., & Schmitz, C. (2011). Recruitment of both the mirror and the mentalizing networks when observing social interactions depicted by point-lights: A neuroimaging study. PLoS ONE, 6(1), e15749.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D., Kraemer, D. J. M., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1383–1392.PubMedCrossRef Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D., Kraemer, D. J. M., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1383–1392.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Research, 1079, 4–14.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Research, 1079, 4–14.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Baillet, S. (1998). A phonological representation in the infant brain. NeuroReport, 9, 1885–1888.PubMedCrossRef Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Baillet, S. (1998). A phonological representation in the infant brain. NeuroReport, 9, 1885–1888.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738.PubMedCrossRef Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Esposito, G., & Venuti, P. (2008). Analysis of toddlers’ gait after six months of independent walking to identify autism: A preliminary study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 259–269.PubMedCrossRef Esposito, G., & Venuti, P. (2008). Analysis of toddlers’ gait after six months of independent walking to identify autism: A preliminary study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 259–269.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Falck-Ytter, T., Gredeback, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878–879.PubMedCrossRef Falck-Ytter, T., Gredeback, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878–879.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Forssberg, H., Eliasson, A. C., Kinoshita, H., Johansson, R. S., & Westling, G. (1991). Development of human precision grip-I—basic coordination of force. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 451–457.CrossRef Forssberg, H., Eliasson, A. C., Kinoshita, H., Johansson, R. S., & Westling, G. (1991). Development of human precision grip-I—basic coordination of force. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 451–457.CrossRef
go back to reference Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218, 486–487.PubMedCrossRef Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218, 486–487.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Haberlen, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., et al. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1480–1494.PubMedCrossRef Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Haberlen, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., et al. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1480–1494.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happe, F. G. E., & Booth, R. D. L. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63.CrossRef Happe, F. G. E., & Booth, R. D. L. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63.CrossRef
go back to reference Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.PubMedCrossRef Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2010b). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 575–583.PubMedCrossRef Heyes, C. (2010b). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 575–583.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hubert, B., Wicker, B., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., Duverger, H., Da Fonseca, D., et al. (2007). Brief report: Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1386–1392.PubMedCrossRef Hubert, B., Wicker, B., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., Duverger, H., Da Fonseca, D., et al. (2007). Brief report: Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1386–1392.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103–S109.PubMedCrossRef Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103–S109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.CrossRef Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, S. A., Blaha, L. M., Houpt, J. W., & Townsend, J. T. (2010). Systems factorial technology provides new insights on global-local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 53–72.CrossRef Johnson, S. A., Blaha, L. M., Houpt, J. W., & Townsend, J. T. (2010). Systems factorial technology provides new insights on global-local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 53–72.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman assessment battery for children (K-ABC). MN: American Guidance Service. Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman assessment battery for children (K-ABC). MN: American Guidance Service.
go back to reference Kilner, J. M. (2009). Dissociable functional roles of the human action-observation network (Commentary on E. S. Cross et al.). European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1382.PubMedCrossRef Kilner, J. M. (2009). Dissociable functional roles of the human action-observation network (Commentary on E. S. Cross et al.). European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1382.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lepage, J. F., & Theoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: Grasping others’ actions from birth? Developmental Science, 10, 513–523.PubMedCrossRef Lepage, J. F., & Theoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: Grasping others’ actions from birth? Developmental Science, 10, 513–523.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. NeuroImage, 56, 1832–1838.PubMedCrossRef Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. NeuroImage, 56, 1832–1838.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Lecouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised—a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Lecouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised—a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2008). The posterior superior temporal sulcus is involved in social communication not specific for the eyes. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2759–2765.PubMedCrossRef Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2008). The posterior superior temporal sulcus is involved in social communication not specific for the eyes. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2759–2765.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McAleer, P., Kay, J., Pollick, F., & Rutherford, M. (2011). Intention perception in high functioning people with autism spectrum disorders using animacy displays derived from human actions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1053–1063.PubMedCrossRef McAleer, P., Kay, J., Pollick, F., & Rutherford, M. (2011). Intention perception in high functioning people with autism spectrum disorders using animacy displays derived from human actions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1053–1063.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.PubMedCrossRef Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, D. G., Hobson, R., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.CrossRef Moore, D. G., Hobson, R., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.CrossRef
go back to reference Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). Conspec and conlern—a 2-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–181.PubMedCrossRef Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). Conspec and conlern—a 2-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., & Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3225–3235.PubMedCrossRef Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., & Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3225–3235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nystrom, P. (2008). The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG. Social Neuroscience, 3, 334–347.PubMedCrossRef Nystrom, P. (2008). The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG. Social Neuroscience, 3, 334–347.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.PubMedCrossRef Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 310–327.PubMedCrossRef Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 310–327.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2008). Preliminary evidence for deficits in multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders: The mirror neuron hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 348–355.PubMedCrossRef Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2008). Preliminary evidence for deficits in multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders: The mirror neuron hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 348–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.PubMedCrossRef Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Parron, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 12, 261–274.PubMedCrossRef Parron, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 12, 261–274.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pavlova, M., Krageloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., & Birbaumer, N. (2001). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception, 30, 925–933.PubMedCrossRef Pavlova, M., Krageloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., & Birbaumer, N. (2001). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception, 30, 925–933.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pellicano, E., & Gibson, L. Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2593–2596.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano, E., & Gibson, L. Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2593–2596.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., Michelich, C. R., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: An FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1866–1876.PubMedCrossRef Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., Michelich, C. R., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: An FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1866–1876.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Plaisted, K., Dobler, V., Bell, S., & Davis, G. (2006). The microgenesis of global perception in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 107–116.PubMedCrossRef Plaisted, K., Dobler, V., Bell, S., & Davis, G. (2006). The microgenesis of global perception in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 107–116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.PubMedCrossRef Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Provost, B., Lopez, B. R., & Heimerl, S. (2007). A comparison of motor delays in young children: Autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental concerns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 321–328.PubMedCrossRef Provost, B., Lopez, B. R., & Heimerl, S. (2007). A comparison of motor delays in young children: Autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental concerns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 321–328.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 264–274.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 264–274.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rochat, P. (2007). Intentional action arises from early reciprocal exchanges. Acta Psychologica, 124, 8–25.PubMedCrossRef Rochat, P. (2007). Intentional action arises from early reciprocal exchanges. Acta Psychologica, 124, 8–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schmitz, C., & Assaiante, C. (2008). Postural control in children with autism spectrum disorder. In M. Hadders-Algra & E. Brogren Carlberg (Eds.), Posture: A key issue in developmental disorders (pp. 253–269). London: Mac Keith Press. Schmitz, C., & Assaiante, C. (2008). Postural control in children with autism spectrum disorder. In M. Hadders-Algra & E. Brogren Carlberg (Eds.), Posture: A key issue in developmental disorders (pp. 253–269). London: Mac Keith Press.
go back to reference Schmitz, C., Martin, N., & Assaiante, C. (2002). Building anticipatory postural adjustment during childhood: A kinematic and electromyographic analysis of unloading in children from 4 to 8 years of age. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 354–364.CrossRef Schmitz, C., Martin, N., & Assaiante, C. (2002). Building anticipatory postural adjustment during childhood: A kinematic and electromyographic analysis of unloading in children from 4 to 8 years of age. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 354–364.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmitz, C., Martineau, J., Barthelemy, C., & Assaiante, C. (2003). Motor control and children with autism: Deficit of anticipatory function? Neuroscience Letters, 348, 17–20.PubMedCrossRef Schmitz, C., Martineau, J., Barthelemy, C., & Assaiante, C. (2003). Motor control and children with autism: Deficit of anticipatory function? Neuroscience Letters, 348, 17–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 809–813.PubMedCrossRef Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 809–813.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.PubMedCrossRef Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.PubMedCrossRef Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tardif, C., Laine, F., Rodriguez, M., & Gepner, B. (2007). Slowing down presentation of facial movements and vocal sounds enhances facial expression recognition and induces facial-vocal imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1469–1484.PubMedCrossRef Tardif, C., Laine, F., Rodriguez, M., & Gepner, B. (2007). Slowing down presentation of facial movements and vocal sounds enhances facial expression recognition and induces facial-vocal imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1469–1484.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C., & Bekkering, H. (2008). You’ll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage, 43, 808–814.PubMedCrossRef van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C., & Bekkering, H. (2008). You’ll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage, 43, 808–814.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vandenbroucke, M. W. G., Scholte, H. S., van Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A. F., & Kemner, C. (2008). Coherent versus component motion perception in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 941–949.PubMedCrossRef Vandenbroucke, M. W. G., Scholte, H. S., van Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A. F., & Kemner, C. (2008). Coherent versus component motion perception in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 941–949.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., et al. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29, 90–98.PubMedCrossRef Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., et al. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29, 90–98.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference von Hofsten, C. (2007). Action in development. Developmental Science, 10, 54–60.CrossRef von Hofsten, C. (2007). Action in development. Developmental Science, 10, 54–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, L. X., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574.PubMedCrossRef Wang, L. X., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.PubMedCrossRef Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1990). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence -revised (WPPSI-R): Short form vocabulary and block design. New York: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1990). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence -revised (WPPSI-R): Short form vocabulary and block design. New York: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1996). Manual for intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1996). Manual for intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.PubMedCrossRef Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zivotofsky, A., & Hausdorff, J. (2007). The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 4, 28.PubMedCrossRef Zivotofsky, A., & Hausdorff, J. (2007). The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 4, 28.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
From Action to Interaction: Exploring the Contribution of Body Motion Cues to Social Understanding in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Auteurs
Laurie Centelles
Christine Assaiante
Katallin Etchegoyhen
Manuel Bouvard
Christina Schmitz
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 5/2013
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2013

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 5/2013 Naar de uitgave