Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2019

07-01-2019 | Original Article

Flexible coupling of covert spatial attention and motor planning based on learned spatial contingencies

Auteurs: David Dignath, Oliver Herbort, Aleksandra Pieczykolan, Lynn Huestegge, Andrea Kiesel

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present study tested whether the coupling of covert attentional shifts and motor planning of pointing movements can be modulated by learning. Participants performed two tasks. As a primary movement task, they executed a pointing movement to a movement target (MT) location. As a secondary visual attention task, they identified a discrimination target (DT) that was presented shortly before initiation of the pointing movement. These DTs either occurred at the same or at different locations with the MT. A common finding in such and similar settings is the enhanced visual target identification when locations of MT and DT coincide. However, it is not known which factors govern the flexibility of spatial attention–action coupling. Here, we tested the influence of previously learned spatial contingencies between MT and DT on the coupling of covert attention and motor planning. These contingencies were manipulated in three groups (always same locations, always opposite locations, non-contingent locations) in a training session. Results indicated that in a subsequent test phase, previously learned contingencies enhanced visual identification accordingly, even when targets for the movement task and the visual task were presented at opposite sides. These results corroborate previous findings of a rather flexible interaction of attention and motor planning, and demonstrate how one can learn to control attention by means of motor planning.
Literatuur
go back to reference Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., Stanley, C. M., Snyder, A. Z., van Essen, D. C., & Corbetta, M. (2003). Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 4689–4699.CrossRefPubMed Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., Stanley, C. M., Snyder, A. Z., van Essen, D. C., & Corbetta, M. (2003). Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 4689–4699.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2010). Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping. Vision Research, 50, 999–1013.CrossRefPubMed Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2010). Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping. Vision Research, 50, 999–1013.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baldauf, D., Wolf, M., & Deubel, H. (2006). Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements. Vision Research, 46, 4355–4374.CrossRefPubMed Baldauf, D., Wolf, M., & Deubel, H. (2006). Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements. Vision Research, 46, 4355–4374.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bressler, S. L., Tang, W., Sylvester, C. M., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2008). Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10056–10061.CrossRefPubMed Bressler, S. L., Tang, W., Sylvester, C. M., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2008). Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10056–10061.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top-down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10, 360–365.CrossRef Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top-down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10, 360–365.CrossRef
go back to reference Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (2003). Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 224–234.PubMed Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (2003). Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 224–234.PubMed
go back to reference Collins, T., Schicke, T., & Röder, B. (2008). Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition, 109, 363–371.CrossRefPubMed Collins, T., Schicke, T., & Röder, B. (2008). Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition, 109, 363–371.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems?. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 831–838.CrossRef Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems?. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 831–838.CrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 292–297.CrossRefPubMed Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 292–297.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–1692.PubMed Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–1692.PubMed
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.CrossRefPubMed Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2003). Delayed saccades, but not delayed manual aiming movements, require visual attention shifts. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1004(1), 289–296.CrossRefPubMed Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2003). Delayed saccades, but not delayed manual aiming movements, require visual attention shifts. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1004(1), 289–296.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2004). Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping. In G. W. Humphreys & M. J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in Action (pp. 69–91). Hove (2004): Psychology Press. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2004). Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping. In G. W. Humphreys & M. J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in Action (pp. 69–91). Hove (2004): Psychology Press.
go back to reference Dore-Mazars, K., Pouget, P., & Beauvillain, C. (2004). Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 69, 67–76.CrossRefPubMed Dore-Mazars, K., Pouget, P., & Beauvillain, C. (2004). Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 69, 67–76.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(1), 22–29.CrossRefPubMed Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(1), 22–29.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.CrossRefPubMed Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fecteau, J. H., Bell, A. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2004). Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92, 1728–1737.CrossRefPubMed Fecteau, J. H., Bell, A. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2004). Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92, 1728–1737.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gherri, E., & Eimer, M. (2009). Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: An electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention. Neuropsychologia, 48, 961–969.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gherri, E., & Eimer, M. (2009). Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: An electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention. Neuropsychologia, 48, 961–969.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Gottlieb, J., Balan, P. F., Oristaglio, J., & Schneider, D. (2009). Task specific computations in attentional maps. Vision Research, 49, 1216–1226.CrossRefPubMed Gottlieb, J., Balan, P. F., Oristaglio, J., & Schneider, D. (2009). Task specific computations in attentional maps. Vision Research, 49, 1216–1226.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.CrossRefPubMed Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Kunde, W. (1999). Location-specific target expectancies in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1127–1141. Hoffmann, J., & Kunde, W. (1999). Location-specific target expectancies in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1127–1141.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2005). Local contextual cuing in visual search. Experimental Psychology, 52, 31–38.CrossRefPubMed Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2005). Local contextual cuing in visual search. Experimental Psychology, 52, 31–38.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Adam, J. J. (2011). Oculomotor interference during manual response preparation: Evidence from the response cueing paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 702–707.CrossRef Huestegge, L., & Adam, J. J. (2011). Oculomotor interference during manual response preparation: Evidence from the response cueing paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 702–707.CrossRef
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Fixation disengagement enhances peripheral perceptual processing: Evidence for a perceptual gap effect. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 631–640.CrossRefPubMed Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Fixation disengagement enhances peripheral perceptual processing: Evidence for a perceptual gap effect. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 631–640.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Kreutzfeldt, M. (2012). Action effects in saccade control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 198–203.CrossRefPubMed Huestegge, L., & Kreutzfeldt, M. (2012). Action effects in saccade control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 198–203.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huestegge, L., Pieczykolan, A., & Koch, I. (2014). Talking while looking: On the encapsulation of output system representations. Cognitive Psychology, 73, 73–91.CrossRef Huestegge, L., Pieczykolan, A., & Koch, I. (2014). Talking while looking: On the encapsulation of output system representations. Cognitive Psychology, 73, 73–91.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2005). Attention in Action: Advances from Cognitive Neuroscience. Hove: Psychology Press.CrossRef Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2005). Attention in Action: Advances from Cognitive Neuroscience. Hove: Psychology Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2011). Independent allocation of attention to eye and hand targets in coordinated eye-hand movements. Psychological Science, 22(3), 339–347.CrossRefPubMed Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2011). Independent allocation of attention to eye and hand targets in coordinated eye-hand movements. Psychological Science, 22(3), 339–347.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jonikaitis, D., Schubert, T., & Deubel, H. (2010). Preparing coordinated eye and hand movements: dual-task costs are not attentional. Journal of Vision, 10(14), 23–23.CrossRefPubMed Jonikaitis, D., Schubert, T., & Deubel, H. (2010). Preparing coordinated eye and hand movements: dual-task costs are not attentional. Journal of Vision, 10(14), 23–23.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Khan, A. Z., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., Salemme, R., Luaute, J., Deubel, H., & Pisella, L. (2009). Parietal damage dissociates saccade planning from presaccadic perceptual facilitation. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 383–387.CrossRefPubMed Khan, A. Z., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., Salemme, R., Luaute, J., Deubel, H., & Pisella, L. (2009). Parietal damage dissociates saccade planning from presaccadic perceptual facilitation. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 383–387.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Khan, A. Z., Song, J. H., & McPeek, R. M. (2011). The eye dominates in guiding attention during simultaneous eye and hand movements. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 9–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Khan, A. Z., Song, J. H., & McPeek, R. M. (2011). The eye dominates in guiding attention during simultaneous eye and hand movements. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 9–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Klapetek, A., Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2016). Attention allocation before antisaccades. Journal of Vision, 16(1), 11–11.CrossRefPubMed Klapetek, A., Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2016). Attention allocation before antisaccades. Journal of Vision, 16(1), 11–11.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. Hum. Neurobiol., 4, 219–227.PubMed Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. Hum. Neurobiol., 4, 219–227.PubMed
go back to reference Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.CrossRefPubMed Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Li, H. H., Barbot, A., & Carrasco, M. (2016). Saccade preparation reshapes sensory tuning. Current Biology, 26(12), 1564–1570.CrossRefPubMed Li, H. H., Barbot, A., & Carrasco, M. (2016). Saccade preparation reshapes sensory tuning. Current Biology, 26(12), 1564–1570.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miller, J. (1988). Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 453–471.PubMed Miller, J. (1988). Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 453–471.PubMed
go back to reference Moehler, T., & Fiehler, K. (2014). Effects of spatial congruency on saccade and visual discrimination performance in a dual-task paradigm. Vision Research, 105, 100–111.CrossRefPubMed Moehler, T., & Fiehler, K. (2014). Effects of spatial congruency on saccade and visual discrimination performance in a dual-task paradigm. Vision Research, 105, 100–111.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Montagnini, A., & Castet, E. (2007). Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–16.CrossRefPubMed Montagnini, A., & Castet, E. (2007). Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–16.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Moore, T., & Fallah, M. (2001). Control of Eye Movements and Spatial Attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, pp. 1273–1276. Moore, T., & Fallah, M. (2001). Control of Eye Movements and Spatial Attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, pp. 1273–1276.
go back to reference Musen, G. (1996). Effects of task demands on implicit memory for object-location associations. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 104–113.CrossRef Musen, G. (1996). Effects of task demands on implicit memory for object-location associations. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 104–113.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (1998). Spatial Attention: Mechanisms and Theories. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in Psychological Science: Vol.2. Biological and Cognitive Aspects (pp. 171–198). East Sussex: Psychology Press. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (1998). Spatial Attention: Mechanisms and Theories. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in Psychological Science: Vol.2. Biological and Cognitive Aspects (pp. 171–198). East Sussex: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2010). Pre-motor theory of attention. Scholarpedia, 5, 6311.CrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2010). Pre-motor theory of attention. Scholarpedia, 5, 6311.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a pre-motor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.CrossRefPubMed Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a pre-motor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
go back to reference Smith, D. T., & Schenk, T. (2012). The premotor theory of attention: time to move on? Neuropsychologia, 50(6), 1104–1114.CrossRefPubMed Smith, D. T., & Schenk, T. (2012). The premotor theory of attention: time to move on? Neuropsychologia, 50(6), 1104–1114.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Song, J. H., & McPeek, R. M. (2009). Eye-hand coordination during target selection in a pop-out visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 2681–2692.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Song, J. H., & McPeek, R. M. (2009). Eye-hand coordination during target selection in a pop-out visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 2681–2692.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Stewart, E. E., & Ma-Wyatt, A. (2015). The spatiotemporal characteristics of the attentional shift relative to a reach. Journal of vision, 15(5), 10–10.CrossRefPubMed Stewart, E. E., & Ma-Wyatt, A. (2015). The spatiotemporal characteristics of the attentional shift relative to a reach. Journal of vision, 15(5), 10–10.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Striemer, C., Locklin, J., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., Pisella, L., & Danckert, J. (2009). Attention for action? Examining the link between attention and visuomotor control deficits in a patient with optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1491–1499.CrossRefPubMed Striemer, C., Locklin, J., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., Pisella, L., & Danckert, J. (2009). Attention for action? Examining the link between attention and visuomotor control deficits in a patient with optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1491–1499.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thompson, K. G., & Bichot, N. P. (2005). A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. Progress in Brain Research, 147, 251–262.PubMed Thompson, K. G., & Bichot, N. P. (2005). A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. Progress in Brain Research, 147, 251–262.PubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centered attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891–891.CrossRefPubMed Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centered attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891–891.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wykowska, A., Schubö, A., & Hommel, B. (2009). How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(6), 1755–1769.PubMed Wykowska, A., Schubö, A., & Hommel, B. (2009). How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(6), 1755–1769.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Flexible coupling of covert spatial attention and motor planning based on learned spatial contingencies
Auteurs
David Dignath
Oliver Herbort
Aleksandra Pieczykolan
Lynn Huestegge
Andrea Kiesel
Publicatiedatum
07-01-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1134-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2019

Psychological Research 3/2019 Naar de uitgave