Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2011

01-09-2011 | Original Article

Feature absence–presence and two theories of lapses of sustained attention

Auteurs: William S. Helton, Paul N. Russell

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We conducted two experiments using search asymmetry to test two theories of sustained attention lapses currently debated in the literature: the mindlessness theory and the resource theory. In the first experiment, participants performed a feature present and a feature absent sustained attention to response task (SART). The mindlessness theory predicts more commission errors in a feature present SART than a feature absent SART, while resource theory predicts the reverse, i.e. more commission errors in a feature absent SART than feature present SART. The latter occurred. In the second study, participants performed a naturalistic scene vigilance task with targets more analogous to a feature present search or more analogous to a feature absent search. The vigilance decrement occurred, with an increase in reaction time to targets over the course of the vigil. The decrement was more pronounced with the targets more analogous to a feature absent search. Overall, the results of both studies support a resource theory of sustained attention lapses, not the mindlessness theory.
Literatuur
go back to reference Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 249–259.CrossRef Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 249–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Buck, L. (1966). Reaction time as a measure of perceptual vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 291–304.PubMedCrossRef Buck, L. (1966). Reaction time as a measure of perceptual vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 291–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance. London: Academic Press. Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance. London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load. Cognition, 111, 345–355.PubMedCrossRef Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load. Cognition, 111, 345–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory based method for investigating influences on switching attention to task unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 1–21.PubMedCrossRef Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory based method for investigating influences on switching attention to task unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 1–21.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greene, M. R., & Oliva, A. (2009). The briefest of glances: The time course of natural scene understanding. Psychological Science, 20, 464–472.PubMedCrossRef Greene, M. R., & Oliva, A. (2009). The briefest of glances: The time course of natural scene understanding. Psychological Science, 20, 464–472.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hancock, P. A., & Hart, S. G. (2002). Defeating terrorism: What can human factors/ergonomics offer? Ergonomics and Design, 10, 6–16.CrossRef Hancock, P. A., & Hart, S. G. (2002). Defeating terrorism: What can human factors/ergonomics offer? Ergonomics and Design, 10, 6–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Hancock, P. A., & Warm, J. S. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Human Factors, 31, 519–537.PubMed Hancock, P. A., & Warm, J. S. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Human Factors, 31, 519–537.PubMed
go back to reference Helton, W. S. (2009). Impulsive responding and the sustained attention to response task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 39–47.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S. (2009). Impulsive responding and the sustained attention to response task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 39–47.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Dember, W. N., Warms, J. S., & Matthews, G. (2000). Optimism, pessimism, and false failure feedback: Effects on vigilance performance. Current Psychology, 18, 311–325.CrossRef Helton, W. S., Dember, W. N., Warms, J. S., & Matthews, G. (2000). Optimism, pessimism, and false failure feedback: Effects on vigilance performance. Current Psychology, 18, 311–325.CrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Hayrynen, L., & Schaeffer, D. (2009a). Sustained attention to local and global target features is different: Performance and tympanic membrane temperature. Brain and Cognition, 71, 9–13.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S., Hayrynen, L., & Schaeffer, D. (2009a). Sustained attention to local and global target features is different: Performance and tympanic membrane temperature. Brain and Cognition, 71, 9–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Hollander, T. D., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Wallart, M., et al. (2005). Signal regularity and the mindlessness model of vigilance. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 249–261.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S., Hollander, T. D., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Wallart, M., et al. (2005). Signal regularity and the mindlessness model of vigilance. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 249–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Kern, R. P., & Walker, D. R. (2009b). Conscious thought and the sustained attention to response task. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 600–607.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S., Kern, R. P., & Walker, D. R. (2009b). Conscious thought and the sustained attention to response task. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 600–607.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Matthews, G., & Warm, J. S. (2009c). Stress state mediation between environmental variables and performance: The case of noise and vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 130, 204–213.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S., Matthews, G., & Warm, J. S. (2009c). Stress state mediation between environmental variables and performance: The case of noise and vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 130, 204–213.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Shaw, T., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., & Hancock, P. A. (2008). Effects of warned and unwarned demand transitions on vigilance performance and stress. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 21, 173–184.CrossRef Helton, W. S., Shaw, T., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., & Hancock, P. A. (2008). Effects of warned and unwarned demand transitions on vigilance performance and stress. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 21, 173–184.CrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., & Warm, J. S. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 129, 18–25.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S., & Warm, J. S. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 129, 18–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helton, W. S., Weil, L., Middlemiss, A., & Sawers, A. (2010). Global interference and spatial uncertainty in the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 77–85.PubMedCrossRef Helton, W. S., Weil, L., Middlemiss, A., & Sawers, A. (2010). Global interference and spatial uncertainty in the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 77–85.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hitchcock, E. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S., Maroney, B. W., & See, J. (1999). Effects of cueing and knowledge of results on workload and boredom in sustained attention. Human Factors, 41, 365–372.PubMedCrossRef Hitchcock, E. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S., Maroney, B. W., & See, J. (1999). Effects of cueing and knowledge of results on workload and boredom in sustained attention. Human Factors, 41, 365–372.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hitchcock, E. M., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Shear, P. K., Tripp, L. D., et al. (2003). Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science, 4, 89–112.CrossRef Hitchcock, E. M., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Shear, P. K., Tripp, L. D., et al. (2003). Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science, 4, 89–112.CrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood, NJ: Prentice Hall.
go back to reference Kantowitz, B. H. (1985). Channels and stages in human information processing: A limited analysis of theory methodology. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 29, 135–174.CrossRef Kantowitz, B. H. (1985). Channels and stages in human information processing: A limited analysis of theory methodology. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 29, 135–174.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaplan, S. (1978). Attention and fascination: The search for cognitive clarity. In S. Kaplan & R. Kaplan (Eds.), Humanscape: Environments for people. Ann Arbor, MI: Ulrich. Kaplan, S. (1978). Attention and fascination: The search for cognitive clarity. In S. Kaplan & R. Kaplan (Eds.), Humanscape: Environments for people. Ann Arbor, MI: Ulrich.
go back to reference Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Towards an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169–182.CrossRef Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Towards an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Kemp, S. (1984). Reaction time to a tone in noise as a function of the signal-to-noise ratio and tone level. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 473–476.CrossRef Kemp, S. (1984). Reaction time to a tone in noise as a function of the signal-to-noise ratio and tone level. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 473–476.CrossRef
go back to reference Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 6–21.CrossRef Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 6–21.CrossRef
go back to reference Mackworth, N. H. (1950). Researches on the measurement of human performance. Medical Research Council Special Report, No. 2680. London: H.M.S.O. (Reprinted from Selected papers in the design and use of control systems, pp. 174–331, by H.W. Sinaiko, Ed., 1961, New York: Dover). Mackworth, N. H. (1950). Researches on the measurement of human performance. Medical Research Council Special Report, No. 2680. London: H.M.S.O. (Reprinted from Selected papers in the design and use of control systems, pp. 174–331, by H.W. Sinaiko, Ed., 1961, New York: Dover).
go back to reference Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M., & Hawkins, K. (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia, 37, 661–670.PubMedCrossRef Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M., & Hawkins, K. (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia, 37, 661–670.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martin, M., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 193–211.CrossRef Martin, M., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 193–211.CrossRef
go back to reference Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., Westerman, S. J., & Stammers, R. B. (2000). Human performance: Cognition stress and individual differences. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., Westerman, S. J., & Stammers, R. B. (2000). Human performance: Cognition stress and individual differences. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference McGrath, J. J. (1963). Some problems of definitions and criteria in the study of vigilance performance. In D. N. Buckner & J. J. McGrath (Eds.), Vigilance: A symposium (pp. 227–237). New York: McGraw-Hill. McGrath, J. J. (1963). Some problems of definitions and criteria in the study of vigilance performance. In D. N. Buckner & J. J. McGrath (Eds.), Vigilance: A symposium (pp. 227–237). New York: McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196–204.PubMedCrossRef McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196–204.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2001). Modeling the shape of the scene: A holistic representation of the spatial envelope. International Journal of Computer Vision, 42, 145–175.CrossRef Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2001). Modeling the shape of the scene: A holistic representation of the spatial envelope. International Journal of Computer Vision, 42, 145–175.CrossRef
go back to reference Parasuraman, R. (1979). Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science, 205, 924–927.PubMedCrossRef Parasuraman, R. (1979). Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science, 205, 924–927.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Quinlan, P. T. (2003). Visual feature integration theory: Past, present, and future. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 643–673.PubMedCrossRef Quinlan, P. T. (2003). Visual feature integration theory: Past, present, and future. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 643–673.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758.PubMedCrossRef Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Santee, J. L., & Egeth, H. E. (1982). Do reaction time and accuracy measure the same aspects of letter recognition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 489–501.PubMedCrossRef Santee, J. L., & Egeth, H. E. (1982). Do reaction time and accuracy measure the same aspects of letter recognition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 489–501.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Scerbo, M. (1998). What’s so boring about vigilance? In R. B. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick, & J. S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 145–166). Washington, DC: APA.CrossRef Scerbo, M. (1998). What’s so boring about vigilance? In R. B. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick, & J. S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 145–166). Washington, DC: APA.CrossRef
go back to reference Scerbo, M. W., Greenwald, C. W., & Sawin, D. A. (1993). The effects of subject-controlled pacing and task type on sustained attention and workload. The Journal of General Psychology, 120, 293–307.CrossRef Scerbo, M. W., Greenwald, C. W., & Sawin, D. A. (1993). The effects of subject-controlled pacing and task type on sustained attention and workload. The Journal of General Psychology, 120, 293–307.CrossRef
go back to reference Schnittger, C., Johannes, S., Arnavaz, A., & Munte, T. F. (1997). Relation of cerebral blood flow velocity and level of vigilance in humans. NeuroReport, 8, 1637–1639.PubMedCrossRef Schnittger, C., Johannes, S., Arnavaz, A., & Munte, T. F. (1997). Relation of cerebral blood flow velocity and level of vigilance in humans. NeuroReport, 8, 1637–1639.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S., & Dember, W. N. (1995). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 230–249.CrossRef See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S., & Dember, W. N. (1995). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 230–249.CrossRef
go back to reference Shaw, T. H., Warm, J. S., Finomore, V., Tripp, L., Matthews, G., Weiler, E., et al. (2009). Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance. Neuroscience Letters, 461, 207–211.PubMedCrossRef Shaw, T. H., Warm, J. S., Finomore, V., Tripp, L., Matthews, G., Weiler, E., et al. (2009). Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance. Neuroscience Letters, 461, 207–211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O’Conner, R., et al. (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 657–690.PubMedCrossRef Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O’Conner, R., et al. (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 657–690.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Teichner, W. H. (1974). The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time on watch. Human Factors, 16, 339–353.PubMed Teichner, W. H. (1974). The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time on watch. Human Factors, 16, 339–353.PubMed
go back to reference Temple, J. G., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Jones, K. S., La Grange, C. M., & Matthews, G. (2000). The effects of signal salience and caffeine on performance, workload and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Human Factors, 42, 183–194.PubMedCrossRef Temple, J. G., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Jones, K. S., La Grange, C. M., & Matthews, G. (2000). The effects of signal salience and caffeine on performance, workload and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Human Factors, 42, 183–194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 15–48. Treisman, A. M., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 15–48.
go back to reference Warm, J. S. (1984). An introduction to vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 1–14). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Warm, J. S. (1984). An introduction to vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 1–14). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
go back to reference Warm, J. S. (1993). Vigilance and target detection. In B. M. Huey & C. D. Wickens (Eds.), Workload transitions: Implications for individual and team performance (pp. 139–170). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Warm, J. S. (1993). Vigilance and target detection. In B. M. Huey & C. D. Wickens (Eds.), Workload transitions: Implications for individual and team performance (pp. 139–170). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
go back to reference Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., & Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors, 50, 433–441.PubMedCrossRef Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., & Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors, 50, 433–441.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 971–978.PubMedCrossRef Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 971–978.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Feature absence–presence and two theories of lapses of sustained attention
Auteurs
William S. Helton
Paul N. Russell
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0316-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2011

Psychological Research 5/2011 Naar de uitgave