Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2014

Open Access 01-01-2014 | Original Paper

Factor Structure of Autistic Traits in Children with ADHD

Auteurs: Joanna Martin, Marian L. Hamshere, Michael C. O’Donovan, Michael Rutter, Anita Thapar

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 1/2014

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often co-occur. Factor analyses of ASD traits in children with and without ASD indicate the presence of social and restrictive–repetitive behaviour (RRB) factors. This study used exploratory factor analyses to determine the structure of ASD traits (assessed using the Social Communication Questionnaire) in children with ADHD. Distinct factors were observed for ‘social’ and ‘rigidity’ traits, corresponding to previous factor analyses in clinical ASD and population samples. This indicates that the split between social-communicative and RRB dimensions is unaffected by ADHD in children. Moreover, the study also finds that there is some overlap across hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and RRB traits in children with ADHD, which merits further investigation.

Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show a high rate of symptom overlap, with a substantial proportion of individuals with one of the conditions also meeting diagnostic criteria for the other (Rommelse et al. 2010). Although historically, diagnostic manuals have not allowed for a joint diagnosis of ADHD and ASD, with an ASD diagnosis trumping and excluding a diagnosis of ADHD (DSM-IV & ICD-10), the observed co-occurrence has prompted changes to these diagnostic exclusions for the DSM-5. Overlap of the two conditions is found at the diagnostic level and at the level of symptoms below diagnostic thresholds in referred children and in the general population (Grzadzinski et al. 2011; Reiersen et al. 2007; Rommelse et al. 2010; Ronald et al. 2008). Moreover, given the high heritability of both conditions (Faraone et al. 2005; Freitag 2006), it is of note that family and twin studies show co-heritability (Lichtenstein et al. 2010; Lundström et al. 2011; Mulligan et al. 2009a; Nijmeijer et al. 2009; Taylor et al. 2012).
In recent years, interest has been growing in exploring the overlap of ADHD and ASD, in terms of associated clinical comorbidities, neuropsychological deficits (e.g. executive functioning) and shared genetic susceptibility. Given the time and cost of performing in-depth interviews and observational diagnostic assessments of ASD, such as the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (Lord et al. 1989, 1994), many studies have relied on questionnaires to measure autistic traits. The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; formerly known as the Autism Screening Questionnaire, ASQ) is a parent-rated questionnaire of ASD behaviours (Berument et al. 1999; Rutter et al. 2003). It is based on the ADI-R (Lord et al. 1994) and has been found to agree well with it on a diagnostic level, although only adequately on an item-by-item basis (Bishop and Norbury 2002). The SCQ is widely used as a screening tool or quantitative measure of autistic traits in samples of children with suspected ASD (Eaves et al. 2006), children with ADHD (Kochhar et al. 2011; Kröger et al. 2011; Mulligan et al. 2009a) or other psychopathology (Pine et al. 2008; Towbin et al. 2005) and also in the general population (Mulligan et al. 2009b). Children diagnosed with ADHD score higher on the SCQ than their siblings or typically developing controls (Kochhar et al. 2011; Mulligan et al. 2009a).
Like the ADI-R, the SCQ can be divided into subscales for the three DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnostic sub-domains of autistic symptoms: social-interaction deficits, communication problems and restrictive–repetitive behaviours (RRBs). There is a growing body of evidence suggesting that dividing ASD symptoms into these sub-domains is meaningful (Happé and Ronald 2008); although the behaviours occur together frequently, both phenotypic and genetic correlations between them are only moderate, suggesting that the three sub-domains are to some extent clinically and genetically separable (Ronald et al. 2006a, b). Nevertheless, the sub-domains appear to cluster more than expected by chance (Ronald et al. 2006b). In a recent population-based twin sample, the authors tested whether different molecular genetic variants, assessed genome-wide, predicted social and ‘non-social’ (i.e. RRB) traits, separately (Ronald et al. 2010). The study did not find evidence of these clinical domains being separate at a molecular genetic level, although only 1–2 genome-wide genetic variants were found to be nominally associated with either sub-domain (without allowing for multiple testing), and these did not replicate in an independent clinical sample of children diagnosed with ASD. These non-significant results are arguably due to the study’s relatively small sample size for a genome-wide association study that requires multiple testing (ranging from N = 372–436 in the high and low trait comparison groups), making it unclear whether the same genetic variants are involved in these sub-domains. Therefore, more familial and molecular genetic studies in larger samples are needed to clarify the extent to which these social and RRB sub-domains are aetiologically related. Interestingly, the overlap of ADHD with ASD symptoms has been demonstrated to occur within all three of these sub-domains, though RRBs have been found to be less frequent than social and communication deficits in children with ADHD (Rommelse et al. 2011).
Factor analyses of autistic traits in clinical ASD and community samples, using a variety of ASD measurement tools, generally indicate that multiple factors account for the observed covariance structure of ASD symptoms and traits (Happé and Ronald 2008; Mandy and Skuse 2008). Likely due to differences in study design, there is little agreement in terms of the specific factors and their composition. However, nearly all factor analytic studies derive at least one factor related to social-communicative features and a separate factor related to ‘non-social’ behaviour or RRBs (Mandy and Skuse 2008). To date, there has been no published factor analysis of autistic traits in a group of children with ADHD and it is not yet known whether the presence of ADHD affects the nature of autistic symptoms in some manner.

Aims of the Study

The main aim of this study was thus to explore the structure of autistic traits (as measured by the SCQ) in a clinical sample of children with ADHD, to determine whether this structure was similar to that found in samples of children with ASD and those from the general population. It was hypothesised that social-communicative traits and RRBs would be accounted for by separate, albeit correlated, factors.
A secondary aim was to explore the relationship between ADHD symptoms and ASD traits in a combined exploratory factor analysis. One previous study suggests that the core diagnostic criteria of ADHD and ASD do not overlap (i.e. ADHD and ASD symptoms load on separate sets of factors) in a general population sample of school children (Ghanizadeh 2010). However, although deemed as common conditions, ADHD and ASD have prevalence rates of <5 % in general population samples, with wide variability in reported prevalence rates in different geographic regions (Polanczyk et al. 2007; Rutter 2005). Therefore, it would be valuable to determine whether there is any overlap in ADHD and ASD symptomatology in a clinical sample of children diagnosed with ADHD (and thus having higher rates of individual symptom presence than the average child in the population).

Methods

Sample

Children aged 5–18 were referred to the study by clinicians from Child and Adolescent Psychiatry or Paediatric clinics in Wales and across the UK. Inclusion criteria were a current diagnosis of DSM-IV or DSM-III-R ADHD, confirmed by a research diagnostic interview (Angold et al. 1995). Exclusion criteria for the original ADHD genetic study (Stergiakouli et al. 2012) were presence of a clinician’s diagnosis of ASD, Tourette’s syndrome, psychosis, epilepsy, bipolar disorder, brain damage or any other neurological or genetic disorder. The sample consisted of 821 children who met inclusion criteria and had data on the SCQ. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the North West England and Wales Multicentre Research Ethics Committees. Parents and children aged 16 years and older provided written informed consent and children under 16 provided assent.

Measures

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder was assessed using the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Assessment (CAPA), a research diagnostic interview undertaken by trained psychologists with the children’s parents, which has well-established high test–retest reliability and construct validity (Angold and Costello 2000; Angold et al. 1995). Parents were asked about presence of each of the 18 ADHD symptoms from the DSM-IV/ICD-10 and about impairment of functioning. The interviewers were supervised weekly by a child psychiatrist and inter-rater reliability for ADHD sub-type research diagnosis, assessed using 60 cases, was perfect (κ = 1.0). Pervasiveness of symptoms, necessary for a DSM-IV diagnosis, was assessed through questionnaires (DuPaul or Conner’s teacher rating scale (Conners et al. 1998; DuPaul 1981)) sent to the school or the Child ADHD Teacher Telephone Interview (ChATTI (Holmes et al. 2004)).
The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) is a 40-item parent-rated questionnaire of autistic traits, with good validity in differentiating children with ASD from those with other learning difficulties and a high correlation with the ADI-R that it is based on (Berument et al. 1999; Rutter et al. 2003). Item 1 of the SCQ is a language screening question and is not included in calculating an overall total of autistic traits. Parents responded to each question by marking ‘yes’ or ‘no’ on the form. Items were re-coded to indicate presence or absence of the autistic behaviour. The 39 items were divided into the three sub-domains of autistic symptoms, as defined by the diagnostic symptoms stipulated by the DSM-IV and ICD-10; there were 20 items classed as ‘social-interaction deficits’, 10 as ‘communication deficits’, 8 as ‘restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs)’ and one item (item 18) was unclassified.
Full-scale IQ was assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III & WISC-IV), using all ten subtests (Wechsler 1992, 2003). Comorbid problems were assessed using the CAPA (Angold et al. 1995).

Factor Analysis

To test the main hypothesis, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed on the 39 SCQ items and the results were compared with a previous factor analysis of the SCQ in a sample of children with ASD and other psychiatric problems (Berument et al. 1999). The secondary aim of the study was addressed by adding in the 18 ADHD symptoms into the EFA model; this analysis is henceforth referred to as the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis.
Cases with any missing data in either analysis were excluded (SCQ analysis: 97 with 1/39, 52 with 2/39 and 112 with >2/39 items missing; combined SCQ-ADHD analysis: 117 with 1/57, 55 with 2/57 and 115 with >2/57 items missing), leaving a complete data set of N = 560 for the SCQ analysis, and N = 534 for the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis. Children included in the analyses did not differ from those with missing data in terms of gender, age at assessment, family socioeconomic status, severity of their ADHD symptoms or presence of oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder or anxiety. They did differ in terms of IQ, with children with missing data having lower IQ than those included (82.4 vs. 84.6, p = 0.045 for children in the SCQ analysis and 82.3 vs. 84.7, p = 0.019 for those in the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis). All variables were dichotomous (symptom presence/absence). For each analysis, bivariate associations were calculated as tetrachoric correlations. The tetrachoric correlation matrices were not positive definite, so a smoothing algorithm was applied to the correlation matrix (using the R command: tetrachoric (“data”, smooth = T)). Visual inspection of the matrices showed extremely high correlation (tetrachoric correlation = 0.95) of SCQ items 24 “when he/she was 4–5 did he/she nod his/her head to mean yes?” and 25 “when he/she was 4–5 did he/she shake his/her head to mean no?”, therefore item 25 was dropped from further analyses.
For each analysis, the correlation matrix was analysed using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to find the minimum residual (minres) solution. This solution was deemed most appropriate given that the assumption of multivariate normality was not fulfilled due to dichotomous variables and the tetrachoric correlation matrices being not positive definite. Choice of number of factors was based on a combination of theory (based on previous literature) and points of inflection on the scree plots. These methods were used in conjunction to maximise variance explained, while maintaining a parsimonious and theory-driven approach towards conceptualising the target construct. For plausible alternative solutions, patterns of correlations across the factor scores were also examined to ascertain the pattern of association across the derived measured factors. The EFA solution was rotated using an oblique rotation (oblimin) as non-independence of the underlying factors was hypothesised (Matsunaga 2010). For ease of interpretation, factor loadings below 0.2 are not shown. All analyses were performed in R.

Factor Scores

To test whether IQ, age, gender or presence of intellectual disability (ID; IQ < 70) had any effects on the results, factor scores were calculated for each analysis. For each factor, a weighted average score for each individual was calculated using all the loadings from the pattern matrices as weights. Pearson correlations were calculated for each factor score with age and IQ. T-tests were used to compare factor scores in (1) boys relative to girls and (2) children with ID relative to those without ID.

Results

ADHD Sample

Demographic information for the sample can be found in Table 1. Additionally, the prevalence of individual ADHD symptoms ranged from 69.9 to 96.6 %. The prevalence of individual SCQ items ranged from 7.9 to 67.3 %.
Table 1
Sample demographic information
Variable
Range
Mean
SD
Age at assessment
5–18
10.4
3.0
Total ADHD symptoms
7–18
15.1
2.5
Total SCQ score
0–35
12.8
6.7
IQ score
46–124
84.6
13.9
 
N
%
Gender
 Male
473
84.5
 Female
87
15.5
Presence of ID
 ID (IQ < 70)
63
11.9
Family socioeconomic status
 Low
259
50.5
 Medium
187
36.5
 High
67
13.1
ADHD diagnosis subtype
 DSM IV combined subtype
405
72.3
 DSM IV predominantly inattentive subtype
28
5.0
 DSM IV predominantly hyperactive-impulsive subtype
60
10.7
 DSM III-R only
67
12.0
ODD or CD diagnosis
 DSM-IV ODD diagnosis
234
44.5
 DSM-IV CD diagnosis
103
18.6
Anxiety or depression diagnosis
 Any DSM-IV anxiety diagnosisa
42
7.8
 Any DSM-IV depression diagnosisa
3
0.6
SCQ social communication questionnaire, ID intellectual disability, ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, ODD oppositional defiant disorder, CD conduct disorder
a2 children met criteria for a diagnosis of both anxiety and depression

SCQ Analysis

The scree plot for the EFA of the SCQ items showed two points of inflection: occurring after 3 and 8 factors, explaining 43.8 and 62.2 % of the variance, respectively. A 3-factor solution was chosen on the basis of this information combined with theory (the DSM-IV & ICD-10 distinguish between three sub-domains of ASD and the majority of previous factor analytic studies suggest that three factors are meaningful (Mandy and Skuse 2008)). Factor 1 was modestly correlated with factors 2 and 3 but factors 2 and 3 were uncorrelated (see Table 2). Table 3 shows the pattern matrix of loadings for the rotated solution, indicating which items load highly on each ASD sub-domain (based on DSM-IV).
Table 2
Factor correlations for both EFA analyses
 
SCQ EFA
SCQ-ADHD EFA
Factor 1—social
Factor 2—rigidity
Factor 3—non-verbal communication
Factor 1—social
Factor 2—rigidity/hyperactivity
SCQ EFA
 Factor 2—rigidity
0.43*
    
 Factor 3—non-verbal communication
0.37*
−0.05
   
SCQ-ADHD EFA
 Factor 1—social
0.98*
0.39*
0.54*
  
 Factor 2—rigidity/hyperactivity
0.47*
0.97*
−0.16*
0.39*
 
 Factor 3—inattentiveness
0.13*
0.17*
−0.12*
0.11*
0.24*
SCQ social communication questionnaire, EFA exploratory factor analysis, ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
* Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
Table 3
Pattern matrix of loadings for factor analysis of SCQ items
SCQ item
DSM-IV sub-domains
Factor 1—social
Factor 2—rigidity
Factor 3—non-verbal communication
34-joins in social games (4/5)
COM
0.78
  
37-positive response to other children (4/5)
SOC
0.77
  
39-plays imaginative games with others (4/5)
COM
0.77
  
36-interest in other children (4/5)
SOC
0.76
  
27-reciprocates smiles (4/5)
SOC
0.73
  
30-wants others to join in (4/5)
SOC
0.69
  
29-shares things (4/5)
SOC
0.67
  
40-plays cooperatively (4/5)
SOC
0.67
  
38-attention without name called (4/5)
SOC
0.66
  
33-range of facial expressions (4/5)
SOC
0.63
  
35-pretend play (4/5)
COM
0.61
 
0.25
31-comforts others (4/5)
SOC
0.60
  
28-shows things to engage interest (4/5)
SOC
0.57
 
0.30
26-looks at faces (4/5)
SOC
0.45
0.28
 
2-talks to be friendly
COM
0.30
 
0.23
10-appropriate facial expressions
SOC
0.21
  
8-repeats things exactly
COM
 
0.81
 
4-uses odd phrases
COM
 
0.80
 
17-repetitive complicated movements
RRB
 
0.72
 
13-interested in parts not whole
RRB
 
0.72
 
7-invents words/phrases
COM
 
0.68
 
16-unusual movements
RRB
 
0.65
 
5-inappropriate questions
SOC
 
0.61
 
9-has rituals
RRB
 
0.55
 
11-uses other’s hand as tool
SOC
 
0.54
 
12-unusual interests
RRB
 
0.54
 
15-unusual sensory interests
RRB
 
0.54
 
6-pronoun reversal
COM
 
0.50
 
14-unusually intense interests
RRB
 
0.48
 
18-self-injures
0.20
0.43
 
19-always carries specific object around
RRB
 
0.37
 
3-can have a conversation
COM
0.30
0.31
0.27
20-has friends
SOC
 
0.22
 
32-uses gestures with sounds/words (4/5)
SOC
  
0.71
24-nods head (4/5)
SOC
  
0.67
22-points to show things (4/5)
SOC
0.27
 
0.66
23-uses gestures (4/5)
SOC
  
0.66
21-copies people’s actions (4/5)
COM
0.29
 
0.40
COM communication sub-domain, SOC social-interaction sub-domain, RRB restrictive and repetitive behaviours sub-domain. To aid interpretation, loadings of < 0.2 are not presented
Factor 1 was comprised of items regarding social-interaction and communication skills and was labelled the ‘social’ factor. Similarly to a previous factor analysis of the SCQ in children with ASD and other psychiatric problems (Berument et al. 1999), this ‘social’ factor is comprised primarily of a similar set of social-interaction items. Factor 2 in the current analysis was comprised of all the RRB items as well as a few of the social and communication items and was labelled as the ‘rigidity’ factor. The majority of items comprising the ‘rigidity’ factor are all of those that constituted two separate factors labelled ‘abnormal language’ and ‘stereotyped behaviour’ in the previous EFA of the SCQ in children with ASD and other psychiatric problems (Berument et al. 1999). Factor 3 contained items to do with gesturing and was labelled the ‘non-verbal communication’ factor. In the factor analysis by Berument et al. (1999), these items clustered with the majority of the social-interaction items in the ‘social’ factor.

Combined SCQ-ADHD Analysis

Next, the 18 ADHD symptoms and 38 SCQ items were analysed together. The scree plot showed two points of inflection: occurring after 3 and 5 factors, explaining 35.3 and 44.6 % of the variance, respectively. The range of factor inter-correlations for the 3-factor solution (see Table 2) was lower than that for the 5-factor solution (r = −0.08–0.46, p = 0.23–p < 0.001), indicating greater coherence between the derived factors of the 3-factor solution relative to the 5-factor solution. On the basis of this information and the ‘parsimony principle’ (Kline 2010) a 3-factor solution was chosen. All three factors were modestly correlated (see Table 2). The rotated factor loadings are shown in Table 4, indicating which items load on which ASD sub-domains (based on DSM-IV). Factor 1 was very similar to the ‘social’ factor of the SCQ analysis, as can be seen both by its composition and correlation with this factor (see Table 2). Likewise, factor 2 of the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis was very similar to the ‘rigidity’ factor of the SCQ analysis, with the addition of the majority of the hyperactive-impulsive ADHD symptoms. Factor 3 comprised the inattentive ADHD symptoms and one of the hyperactive symptoms. These factors were labelled ‘social’, ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ and ‘inattentiveness’, respectively.
Table 4
Pattern matrix of loadings for factor analysis of SCQ items and ADHD symptoms
 
DSM-IV sub-domains
Factor 1—social
Factor 2—rigidity/hyperactivity
Factor 3—inattentiveness
SCQ 30-wants others to join in (4/5)
SOC
0.75
  
SCQ 39-plays imaginative games with others (4/5)
COM
0.73
  
SCQ 34-joins in social games (4/5)
COM
0.72
  
SCQ 27-reciprocates smiles (4/5)
SOC
0.70
  
SCQ 28-shows things to engage interest (4/5)
SOC
0.69
  
SCQ 35-pretend play (4/5)
COM
0.69
  
SCQ 29-shares things (4/5)
SOC
0.64
  
SCQ 22-points to show things (4/5)
SOC
0.63
  
SCQ 36-interest in other children (4/5)
SOC
0.62
0.25
 
SCQ 31-comforts others (4/5)
SOC
0.62
  
SCQ 37-positive response to other children (4/5)
SOC
0.61
0.29
 
SCQ 38-attention without name called (4/5)
SOC
0.58
0.22
 
SCQ 40-plays cooperatively (4/5)
SOC
0.57
0.28
 
SCQ 33-range of facial expressions (4/5)
SOC
0.56
  
SCQ 26-looks at faces (4/5)
SOC
0.50
0.28
 
SCQ 32-uses gestures with sounds/words (4/5)
SOC
0.50
  
SCQ 21-copies people’s actions (4/5)
COM
0.46
  
SCQ 3-can have a conversation
COM
0.42
0.28
 
SCQ 2-talks to be friendly
COM
0.41
  
SCQ 24-nods head (4/5)
SOC
0.34
  
SCQ 20-has friends
SOC
0.30
  
SCQ 23-uses gestures (4/5)
SOC
0.22
  
SCQ 10-appropriate facial expressions
SOC
0.21
  
SCQ 13-interested in parts not whole
RRB
 
0.74
 
SCQ 8-repeats things exactly
COM
 
0.69
 
SCQ 17-repetitive complicated movements
RRB
 
0.66
 
SCQ 4-uses odd phrases
COM
 
0.66
 
SCQ 7-invents words/phrases
COM
 
0.65
 
SCQ 11-uses other’s hand as tool
SOC
 
0.63
 
SCQ 16-unusual movements
RRB
 
0.62
 
SCQ 12-unusual interests
RRB
 
0.58
 
SCQ 9-has rituals
RRB
 
0.56
 
SCQ 15-unusual sensory interests
RRB
 
0.56
 
SCQ 18-self-injures
 
0.51
 
ADHD IMP 4-talking excessively
IMP
 
0.50
 
SCQ 5-inappropriate questions
SOC
 
0.49
 
SCQ 14-unusually intense interests
RRB
 
0.49
 
ADHD HYP 3-running excessively
HYP
 
0.47
 
SCQ 6-pronoun reversal
COM
 
0.41
 
ADHD HYP 5-often noisy
HYP
 
0.39
0.36
ADHD HYP 4-on the go
HYP
 
0.37
 
SCQ 19-always carries specific object around
RRB
 
0.37
 
ADHD HYP 1-fidgeting
HYP
 
0.37
0.29
ADHD IMP 1-waiting for turn
IMP
 
0.32
0.27
ADHD IMP 2-blurting out answers
IMP
 
0.26
 
ADHD IMP 3-interrupting
IMP
 
0.23
0.23
ADHD IN 1-sustaining attention
INA
  
0.74
ADHD IN 7-making careless mistakes
INA
  
0.72
ADHD IN 2-following instructions
INA
  
0.71
ADHD IN 8-forgetful
INA
  
0.69
ADHD IN 9-organisational difficulty
INA
  
0.65
ADHD IN 3-avoiding mental effort
INA
  
0.62
ADHD IN 6-not listening
INA
  
0.60
ADHD IN 4-easily distracted
INA
  
0.55
ADHD IN 5-losing things
INA
  
0.44
ADHD HYP 2-remaining seated
HYP
 
0.29
0.41
COM communication sub-domain, SOC social-interaction sub-domain, RRB restrictive and repetitive behaviours sub-domain, IMP impulsive symptoms, HYP hyperactive symptoms, INA inattentive symptoms. To aid interpretation, loadings of < 0.2 are not presented
It can be seen from Table 2 that all the factors from the SCQ analysis are significantly correlated with those from the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis. The majority are positively correlated, with the exception of the SCQ ‘non-verbal communication’ factor with the SCQ-ADHD ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ and ‘inattentiveness’ factors (those comprising ADHD symptoms), which were negatively correlated.

Correlates of Factor Scores

Age at assessment was negatively correlated with the ‘rigidity’ factor of the SCQ analysis and the ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ factor of the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis and positively correlated with the ‘non-verbal communication’ factor of the SCQ analysis. In other words, younger children showed higher rates of RRBs and hyperactive-impulsive behaviours, whereas older children showed more of some of the social-communication ASD traits. Correlation results are displayed in Table 5. The factor scores from both analyses were negatively correlated with IQ (p < 0.05), except for the ‘inattentiveness’ factor from the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis, which only showed a trend towards a negative correlation (p = 0.09).
Table 5
Factor score correlations with age and IQ
Factor
Age
IQ
SCQ EFA
 Factor 1—social
0.01
−0.21**
 Factor 2—rigidity
−0.13**
−0.12**
 Factor 3—non-verbal communication
0.12**
−0.13**
SCQ-ADHD EFA
 Factor 1—social
0.05
−0.21**
 Factor 2—rigidity/hyperactivity
−0.20**
−0.10*
 Factor 3—inattentiveness
−0.03
−0.08
* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
Children with ID (IQ < 70) had higher factor scores for the first two factors of either analysis (p < 0.05), i.e. the ‘social’, ‘rigidity’ and ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ factors. These results are displayed in Table 6. In terms of gender, boys tended to have higher scores than girls (p < 0.05) on the ‘social’ factor of the SCQ analysis. There were no gender differences for the other factors (see Table 6).
Table 6
Association of factor scores with gender and presence of ID
Factor
Gender
Mean (SD)
t
p
Presence of ID
Mean (SD)
t
p
SCQ EFA
Factor 1—social
F
0.25 (0.19)
−2.18
0.03
No ID
0.28 (0.22)
−3.95
<0.001
M
0.30 (0.23)
ID
0.40 (0.25)
Factor 2—rigidity
F
0.40 (0.22)
0.25
0.80
No ID
0.37 (0.24)
−2.81
0.01
M
0.39 (0.25)
ID
0.46 (0.27)
Factor 3—non-verbal communication
F
0.32 (0.28)
−0.25
0.80
No ID
0.32 (0.28)
−1.51
0.13
M
0.33 (0.29)
ID
0.38 (0.32)
SCQ-ADHD EFA
Factor 1—social
F
0.24 (0.20)
−1.61
0.11
No ID
0.26 (0.21)
−3.49
<0.001
M
0.28 (0.22)
ID
0.37 (0.23)
Factor 2—rigidity/hyperactivity
F
0.50 (0.19)
−0.39
0.70
No ID
0.49 (0.21)
−2.44
0.01
M
0.51 (0.22)
ID
0.57 (0.23)
Factor 3—inattentiveness
F
0.89 (0.18)
0.54
0.59
No ID
0.88 (0.19)
−1.36
0.17
M
0.88 ( 0.19)
ID
0.91 (0.17)
ID intellectual disability (IQ < 70)

Discussion

In children with ADHD, the results of the SCQ factor analysis indicate that items tapping into social deficits and RRBs constitute mainly separate factors, with various items for communication deficits clustering with one or the other factor. In comparison to a previous factor analysis of the SCQ in children with ASD and other psychiatric problems (Berument et al. 1999), the largest factor is comprised of social-interaction items, whereas items concerning RRBs constitute a separate factor, which in this previous EFA were sub-divided into two separate factors labelled ‘abnormal language’ and ‘stereotyped behaviour’. Although there are differences in which specific items are included in the different factors in the current analysis compared to the one by Berument and colleagues (1999), the similarities are striking.
Moreover, the results suggest that the factor structure of autistic traits (measured by the SCQ) in children with ADHD is comparable to previous reports of factor analyses of autistic measures in children with ASD and the general population showing separate social and ‘non-social’ or RRB factors (Happé and Ronald 2008; Mandy and Skuse 2008). It is important to note though, that despite these separate clusters of items, these factors (in particular the ‘social’ and ‘rigidity’ factors) are correlated, indicating that they are not independent of each other. Although the symptoms reported here are not at diagnostic levels, these results also support the growing body of evidence for changing ASD diagnostic criteria from a triad (social-interaction deficits, communication problems and RRBs) to a dyad (social-communication deficits and RRBs) of symptom types in the DSM-5 (Frazier et al. 2012; Mandy et al. 2012).
The second analysis, which explored whether ADHD symptoms and items of the SCQ group together or form separate factors, found that the two factors of items of social-communicative deficits from the SCQ factor analysis (i.e. the ‘social’ and ‘non-verbal communication’ factors) combined into a single ‘social’ factor and the hyperactive-impulsive symptoms tended to group with RRB items in a ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ factor, with a separate factor for inattentive ADHD symptoms. Although there are minor differences in which items load more strongly on the specific factors in this analysis, compared to the analysis of the SCQ items only, the division of the ASD traits into ‘social’ and ‘rigidity’ factors appears unaffected by including ADHD symptoms in the analysis.
The observed division of the ADHD symptoms into separate factors with hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive symptoms is well-supported in the ADHD literature (Willcutt et al. 2012). Indeed, as with social-communicative symptoms relative to RRBs (Ronald et al. 2006a, b, 2010), there is evidence suggesting that hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive symptoms show some level of genetic heterogeneity and specificity in addition to substantial genetic overlap (Greven et al. 2011). It is of particular interest that the hyperactive-impulsive ADHD items clustered with the RRB items and we can only speculate as to why this might be the case. This finding requires further investigation and replication in other samples.
There is growing evidence that ADHD and ASD are each the extreme end of a continuum rather than being distinct categories (Constantino and Todd 2003; Larsson et al. 2011; Levy et al. 1997). Given this, the division of ADHD into two dimensional scales of inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and of ASD into social-communication and RRB dimensions has important implications for classification of developmental problems, investigating the aetiology of these traits and understanding the high heritability and co-heritability of the two disorders (Faraone et al. 2005; Freitag 2006; Lichtenstein et al. 2010).
Although a 3-factor solution for the combined SCQ-ADHD analysis is believed to be the optimal solution, an alternative was to choose a less parsimonious 5-factor solution (full results available from the first author). This solution bears many similarities to the main analyses. The primary factor is composed of a highly similar set of items to the SCQ ‘social’ and the SCQ-ADHD 3-factor ‘social’ factors and is very highly correlated with these (Pearson correlations of 0.99 and 0.98, respectively). Similarly, the second factor of the 5-factor SCQ-ADHD solution is very highly correlated with the ‘rigidity’ SCQ factor (0.995) and the ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ factor (0.98) of the SCQ-ADHD 3-factor analysis. The third factor appears to correspond to the ‘inattentiveness’ factor of the SCQ-ADHD 3-factor analysis (correlation = 0.98). The fourth factor contains primarily hyperactive and impulsive ADHD symptoms and the fifth factor corresponds to the ‘non-verbal communication’ factor of the SCQ analysis (correlation = 0.96). The key point about the 5-factor solution is that the ASD items come out separately to the ADHD symptoms (i.e. it has three factors of ASD items corresponding to the SCQ analysis and two separate factors for ADHD symptoms). Such a solution is in line with the previous exploratory factor analysis of core ADHD and ASD diagnostic criteria in a population sample of school children (Ghanizadeh 2010). However, it is important to consider competing factor solutions and further studies are needed to clarify the extent of the overlap of RRBs and hyperactive-impulsive ADHD symptoms. It would also be worth exploring the factor structure of ADHD and ASD symptoms in children diagnosed with ASD. One small study (N = 65) has attempted to do this, and although they found distinct factors for ASD and ADHD, they do not consider competing models and provide no clear justification for the choice of a 2-factor solution (Ghanizadeh 2012).
Although children with ID (IQ < 70) tend to be excluded from studies of ADHD and sometimes also of ASD, these children were included in the present analyses (N = 63). Given the high association of lower IQ and higher rates of ID in these neurodevelopmental conditions (Frazier et al. 2004; Voigt et al. 2006), IQ is not statistically separable from neurodevelopmental problems (Dennis et al. 2009) and the deliberate recruitment of children without ID may bias representativeness of such samples. Indeed, analysis of factor scores in relation to IQ showed that the factor scores were negatively correlated with IQ, indicating that children with the most severe symptom profiles were likely to score lower on the IQ test. There is also evidence that children with ADHD and comorbid ID do not differ in their ADHD profile to those with ADHD without ID (Ahuja et al. 2013; Antshel et al. 2006). A complete re-analysis of the data excluding the children with ID shows no marked differences to the pattern of observed results (available from first author upon request).
Given that ADHD and ASD are developmental conditions, it is not surprising that some of the factor scores showed associations with age. There appeared to be no effect for age for the primary ‘social’ factors, whereas the ‘rigidity’ and ‘rigidity/hyperactivity’ factor scores decreased with age and the ‘non-verbal communication’ factor scores increased with age. Although it is well-established that hyperactivity and impulsivity decrease with age (Willcutt et al. 2012), it is less clear why older children would struggle more on the items comprising the ‘non-verbal communication’ factor, unless this is related to parental recall of items from when the children were aged 4–5.
In terms of gender, boys had higher scores than girls on the ‘social’ factor of the SCQ analysis but boys and girls did not differ on the other factor scores. Given that there is a high ratio of boys to girls in samples of children with ADHD and ASD, it is reasonable that boys with ADHD are more likely to have higher ASD scores than girls, although it is unclear why this is the case only for the social difficulties. Limiting the analysis to boys-only makes no difference to the pattern of observed results (details available upon request).

Study Limitations and Conclusions

The results of this study need to be considered in light of several limitations. Data on ADHD and ASD were derived from different types of instruments. Whilst ADHD symptoms were measured using a diagnostic interview completed with parents (Angold et al. 1995), ASD traits were measured using a questionnaire measure (Rutter et al. 2003). Future studies will need to examine whether the pattern of results is affected by the type of instrument used.
Although the relatively large sample size was a strength of the current study, it was not sufficiently large enough to analyse stratified age groups. Also, parental recall of their children’s behaviour (particularly at ages 4–5) might have been different for adolescents. The findings of this study relate to clinic children diagnosed with ADHD and it is possible that the observed association of hyperactive-impulsive items with RRBs might not be evident in children with lower levels of such traits, as has been suggested by one other study (Ghanizadeh 2010).
However, despite these caveats, the study contributes novel findings to the growing body of literature exploring the overlap of ADHD and ASD. The results highlight that there are distinct dimensions of social-communicative difficulties and RRBs in children diagnosed with ADHD, supporting such a finding in children with ASD and in the general population (Mandy and Skuse 2008). This finding further implies that the presence or absence of ADHD does not affect the manifestation of social-communicative difficulties and RRBs in children. The results also support the switch from a triad to a dyad of diagnostic symptom dimensions in the DSM-5 (Frazier et al. 2012; Mandy et al. 2012). The suggestion that hyperactive-impulsive traits may be linked with RRBs is an intriguing one and requires replication and further study, both in clinically referred children and in the general population.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Professor Gordon Harold for advice on choice and interpretation of factor solutions as well as proof-reading the manuscript and to Professor Dirk Enzmann for assistance with choosing appropriate R script for the method of analysis. We are very grateful to the families, paediatricians and CAMHS clinicians who supported this project. We thank field team members for sample collection. This study was funded by Baily Thomas Charitable Trust, Action Medical Research and the Wellcome Trust.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2000). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 39–48.PubMedCrossRef Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2000). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 39–48.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Angold, A., Prendergast, M., Cox, A., Harrington, R., Simonoff, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Psychological Medicine, 25(4), 739–753.PubMedCrossRef Angold, A., Prendergast, M., Cox, A., Harrington, R., Simonoff, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Psychological Medicine, 25(4), 739–753.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Antshel, K. M., Phillips, M. H., Gordon, M., Barkley, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Is ADHD a valid disorder in children with intellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review, 26(5), 555–572.PubMedCrossRef Antshel, K. M., Phillips, M. H., Gordon, M., Barkley, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Is ADHD a valid disorder in children with intellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review, 26(5), 555–572.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451.PubMedCrossRef Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(7), 917–929.PubMedCrossRef Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(7), 917–929.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N. (1998). Revision and restandardization of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(4), 279–291.PubMedCrossRef Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N. (1998). Revision and restandardization of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(4), 279–291.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 524–530.PubMedCrossRef Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 524–530.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(03), 331–343.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(03), 331–343.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference DuPaul, G. (1981). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 245–253.CrossRef DuPaul, G. (1981). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 245–253.CrossRef
go back to reference Eaves, L. C., Wingert, H. D., Ho, H. H., & Mickelson, E. C. R. (2006). Screening for autism spectrum disorders with the social communication questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S95–S103.PubMedCrossRef Eaves, L. C., Wingert, H. D., Ho, H. H., & Mickelson, E. C. R. (2006). Screening for autism spectrum disorders with the social communication questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S95–S103.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., et al. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1313–1323.PubMedCrossRef Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., et al. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1313–1323.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frazier, T. W., Demaree, H. A., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2004). Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 543–555.PubMedCrossRef Frazier, T. W., Demaree, H. A., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2004). Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 543–555.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P., Constantino, J., et al. (2012). Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(1), 28–40e23, doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2011.09.021. Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P., Constantino, J., et al. (2012). Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(1), 28–40e23, doi:10.​1016/​j.​jaac.​2011.​09.​021.
go back to reference Freitag, C. M. (2006). The genetics of autistic disorders and its clinical relevance: A review of the literature. Molecular Psychiatry, 12(1), 2–22.PubMedCrossRef Freitag, C. M. (2006). The genetics of autistic disorders and its clinical relevance: A review of the literature. Molecular Psychiatry, 12(1), 2–22.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ghanizadeh, A. (2010). Factor analysis on ADHD and autism spectrum disorder DSM-IV-derived items shows lack of overlap. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(10), 797–798.PubMedCrossRef Ghanizadeh, A. (2010). Factor analysis on ADHD and autism spectrum disorder DSM-IV-derived items shows lack of overlap. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(10), 797–798.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ghanizadeh, A. (2012). Co-morbidity and factor analysis on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder DSM-IV-derived items. Journal of Research in Medical Sciences, 17(4), 368.PubMedCentralPubMed Ghanizadeh, A. (2012). Co-morbidity and factor analysis on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder DSM-IV-derived items. Journal of Research in Medical Sciences, 17(4), 368.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Greven, C. U., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2011). A twin study of ADHD symptoms in early adolescence: Hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattentiveness show substantial genetic overlap but also genetic specificity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(2), 265–275.PubMedCrossRef Greven, C. U., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2011). A twin study of ADHD symptoms in early adolescence: Hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattentiveness show substantial genetic overlap but also genetic specificity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(2), 265–275.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grzadzinski, R., Di Martino, A., Brady, E., Mairena, M. A., O’Neale, M., Petkova, E., et al. (2011). Examining autistic traits in children with ADHD: Does the autism spectrum extend to ADHD? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(9), 1178–1191.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Grzadzinski, R., Di Martino, A., Brady, E., Mairena, M. A., O’Neale, M., Petkova, E., et al. (2011). Examining autistic traits in children with ADHD: Does the autism spectrum extend to ADHD? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(9), 1178–1191.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happé, F., & Ronald, A. (2008). The ‘fractionable autism triad’: A review of evidence from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychology Review, 18(4), 287–304.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F., & Ronald, A. (2008). The ‘fractionable autism triad’: A review of evidence from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychology Review, 18(4), 287–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Holmes, J., Lawson, D., Langley, K., Fitzpatrick, H., Trumper, A., Pay, H., et al. (2004). The child attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder teacher telephone interview (CHATTI): Reliability and validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 74–78.PubMedCrossRef Holmes, J., Lawson, D., Langley, K., Fitzpatrick, H., Trumper, A., Pay, H., et al. (2004). The child attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder teacher telephone interview (CHATTI): Reliability and validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 74–78.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford press. Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford press.
go back to reference Kochhar, P., Batty, M. J., Liddle, E. B., Groom, M. J., Scerif, G., Liddle, P. F., et al. (2011). Autistic spectrum disorder traits in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child: Care, Health and Development, 37(1), 103–110. Kochhar, P., Batty, M. J., Liddle, E. B., Groom, M. J., Scerif, G., Liddle, P. F., et al. (2011). Autistic spectrum disorder traits in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child: Care, Health and Development, 37(1), 103–110.
go back to reference Kröger, A., Hänig, S., Seitz, C., Palmason, H., Meyer, J., & Freitag, C. M. (2011). Risk factors of autistic symptoms in children with ADHD. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(11–12), 561–570. doi:10.1007/s00787-011-0221-1.PubMedCrossRef Kröger, A., Hänig, S., Seitz, C., Palmason, H., Meyer, J., & Freitag, C. M. (2011). Risk factors of autistic symptoms in children with ADHD. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(11–12), 561–570. doi:10.​1007/​s00787-011-0221-1.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Larsson, H., Anckarsater, H., Råstam, M., Chang, Z., & Lichtenstein, P. (2011). Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: A quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 73–80.PubMedCrossRef Larsson, H., Anckarsater, H., Råstam, M., Chang, Z., & Lichtenstein, P. (2011). Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: A quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 73–80.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(6), 737–744.PubMedCrossRef Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(6), 737–744.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lichtenstein, P., Carlström, E., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., & Anckarsäter, H. (2010). The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), 1357–1363.PubMedCrossRef Lichtenstein, P., Carlström, E., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., & Anckarsäter, H. (2010). The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), 1357–1363.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 185–212.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 185–212.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lundström, S., Chang, Z., Kerekes, N., Gumpert, C. H., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (2011). Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults. Psychological Medicine, 41(11), 2423–2433.PubMedCrossRef Lundström, S., Chang, Z., Kerekes, N., Gumpert, C. H., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (2011). Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults. Psychological Medicine, 41(11), 2423–2433.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mandy, W. P. L., & Skuse, D. H. (2008). Research Review: What is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 795–808.PubMedCrossRef Mandy, W. P. L., & Skuse, D. H. (2008). Research Review: What is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 795–808.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyze your data right: Do’s, don’ts, and how-to’s. International Journal of Psychological research, 3(1), 97–110. Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyze your data right: Do’s, don’ts, and how-to’s. International Journal of Psychological research, 3(1), 97–110.
go back to reference Mulligan, A., Anney, R. J., O’Regan, M., Chen, W., Butler, L., Fitzgerald, M., et al. (2009a). Autism symptoms in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A familial trait which correlates with conduct, oppositional defiant, language and motor disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 197–209.PubMedCrossRef Mulligan, A., Anney, R. J., O’Regan, M., Chen, W., Butler, L., Fitzgerald, M., et al. (2009a). Autism symptoms in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A familial trait which correlates with conduct, oppositional defiant, language and motor disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 197–209.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mulligan, A., Richardson, T., Anney, R. J. L., & Gill, M. (2009b). The Social Communication Questionnaire in a sample of the general population of school-going children. Irish Journal of Medical Science, 178(2), 193–199.PubMedCrossRef Mulligan, A., Richardson, T., Anney, R. J. L., & Gill, M. (2009b). The Social Communication Questionnaire in a sample of the general population of school-going children. Irish Journal of Medical Science, 178(2), 193–199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nijmeijer, J. S., Hoekstra, P., Minderaa, R., Buitelaar, J., Altink, M., Buschgens, C. J. M., et al. (2009). PDD symptoms in ADHD, an independent familial trait? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(3), 443–453.PubMedCrossRef Nijmeijer, J. S., Hoekstra, P., Minderaa, R., Buitelaar, J., Altink, M., Buschgens, C. J. M., et al. (2009). PDD symptoms in ADHD, an independent familial trait? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(3), 443–453.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pine, D. S., Guyer, A. E., Goldwin, M., Towbin, K. A., & Leibenluft, E. (2008). Autism spectrum disorder scale scores in pediatric mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(6), 652–661.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Pine, D. S., Guyer, A. E., Goldwin, M., Towbin, K. A., & Leibenluft, E. (2008). Autism spectrum disorder scale scores in pediatric mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(6), 652–661.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942.PubMedCrossRef Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reiersen, A. M., Constantino, J. N., Volk, H. E., & Todd, R. D. (2007). Autistic traits in a population based ADHD twin sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(5), 464–472.PubMedCrossRef Reiersen, A. M., Constantino, J. N., Volk, H. E., & Todd, R. D. (2007). Autistic traits in a population based ADHD twin sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(5), 464–472.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rommelse, N. N. J., Franke, B., Geurts, H. M., Hartman, C. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(3), 281–295.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Rommelse, N. N. J., Franke, B., Geurts, H. M., Hartman, C. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(3), 281–295.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rommelse, N. N. J., Geurts, H. M., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J. K., & Hartman, C. A. (2011). A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(6), 1363–1396.PubMedCrossRef Rommelse, N. N. J., Geurts, H. M., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J. K., & Hartman, C. A. (2011). A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(6), 1363–1396.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ronald, A., Bolton, P., Butcher, L. E. E. M., Price, T. S., Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2006a). Genetic heterogeneity between the three components of the autism spectrum: A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(6), 691–699.PubMedCrossRef Ronald, A., Bolton, P., Butcher, L. E. E. M., Price, T. S., Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2006a). Genetic heterogeneity between the three components of the autism spectrum: A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(6), 691–699.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ronald, A., Butcher, L. M., Docherty, S., Davis, O. S. P., Schalkwyk, L. C., Craig, I. W., et al. (2010). A genome-wide association study of social and non-social autistic-like traits in the general population using pooled DNA, 500K SNP microarrays and both community and diagnosed autism replication samples. Behavior Genetics, 40(1), 31–45.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Ronald, A., Butcher, L. M., Docherty, S., Davis, O. S. P., Schalkwyk, L. C., Craig, I. W., et al. (2010). A genome-wide association study of social and non-social autistic-like traits in the general population using pooled DNA, 500K SNP microarrays and both community and diagnosed autism replication samples. Behavior Genetics, 40(1), 31–45.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ronald, A., Price, T. S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Plomin, R. (2006b). Phenotypic and genetic overlap between autistic traits at the extremes of the general population. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(10), 1206–1214.PubMedCrossRef Ronald, A., Price, T. S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Plomin, R. (2006b). Phenotypic and genetic overlap between autistic traits at the extremes of the general population. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(10), 1206–1214.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ronald, A., Simonoff, E., Kuntsi, J., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2008). Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 535–542.PubMedCrossRef Ronald, A., Simonoff, E., Kuntsi, J., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2008). Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 535–542.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M. (2005). Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: Changes over time and their meaning*. Acta Paediatrica, 94(1), 2–15.PubMedCrossRef Rutter, M. (2005). Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: Changes over time and their meaning*. Acta Paediatrica, 94(1), 2–15.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Lord, C., & Berument, S. K. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Lord, C., & Berument, S. K. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Stergiakouli, E., Hamshere, M., Holmans, P., Langley, K., Zaharieva, I., Hawi, Z., et al. (2012). Investigating the contribution of common genetic variants to the risk and pathogenesis of ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(2), 186–194.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Stergiakouli, E., Hamshere, M., Holmans, P., Langley, K., Zaharieva, I., Hawi, Z., et al. (2012). Investigating the contribution of common genetic variants to the risk and pathogenesis of ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(2), 186–194.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Taylor, M. J., Charman, T., Robinson, E. B., Plomin, R., Happé, F., Asherson, P., et al. (2012). Developmental associations between traits of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A genetically informative, longitudinal twin study. Psychological Medicine, 1(1), 1–12. Taylor, M. J., Charman, T., Robinson, E. B., Plomin, R., Happé, F., Asherson, P., et al. (2012). Developmental associations between traits of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A genetically informative, longitudinal twin study. Psychological Medicine, 1(1), 1–12.
go back to reference Towbin, K. E., Pradella, A., Gorrindo, T., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2005). Autism spectrum traits in children with mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(3), 452–464.PubMedCrossRef Towbin, K. E., Pradella, A., Gorrindo, T., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2005). Autism spectrum traits in children with mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(3), 452–464.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Voigt, R. G., Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., & Katusic, S. K. (2006). Developmental dissociation, deviance, and delay: Occurrence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in individuals with and without borderline to mild intellectual disability. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48(10), 831–835.PubMedCrossRef Voigt, R. G., Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., & Katusic, S. K. (2006). Developmental dissociation, deviance, and delay: Occurrence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in individuals with and without borderline to mild intellectual disability. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48(10), 831–835.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (3rd ed.). London: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (3rd ed.). London: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Association. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Association.
go back to reference Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 991–1010. doi:10.1037/a0027347.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 991–1010. doi:10.​1037/​a0027347.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Factor Structure of Autistic Traits in Children with ADHD
Auteurs
Joanna Martin
Marian L. Hamshere
Michael C. O’Donovan
Michael Rutter
Anita Thapar
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1865-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2014

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2014 Naar de uitgave