Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2011

01-01-2011 | Original Article

Eye movements during language-mediated visual search reveal a strong link between overt visual attention and lexical processing in 36-month-olds

Auteurs: Elizabeth K. Johnson, Falk Huettig

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The nature of children’s early lexical processing was investigated by asking what information 36-month-olds access and use when instructed to find a known but absent referent. Children readily retrieved stored knowledge about characteristic color, i.e., when asked to find an object with a typical color (e.g., strawberry), children tended to fixate more upon an object that had the same (e.g., red plane) as opposed to a different (e.g., yellow plane) color. They did so regardless of the fact that they had plenty of time to recognize the pictures for what they are, i.e., planes and not strawberries. These data represent the first demonstration that language-mediated shifts of overt attention in young children can be driven by individual stored visual attributes of known words that mismatch on most other dimensions. The finding suggests that lexical processing and overt attention are strongly linked from an early age.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
We are aware of two exceptions. Saylor (2004) used off-line tasks to ask toddlers about recently labeled missing referents when both spatial and color cues to the missing referent were present. Saylor’s study was designed to test whether toddlers exhibited comprehension of missing referents when multiple visual cues to that referent were available. It was not designed to explore the content or attention driving properties of early lexical representations. Naigles and Gelman (1995) presented children with two animals (e.g., cow and cat) and asked children to find a third (e.g., dog). Their study was designed to study the relationships between overextensions in production and comprehension.
 
2
The difference between color-matched distractor trials and target trials was more statistically reliable during the time window ranging from 3,000 to 4,000 ms after target onset (t (40) = 2.06, p = 0.046).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bauer, P. J., & Mandler, J. M. (1989). Taxonomies and triads: Conceptual organization in one- and two-year-olds. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 156–184.CrossRef Bauer, P. J., & Mandler, J. M. (1989). Taxonomies and triads: Conceptual organization in one- and two-year-olds. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 156–184.CrossRef
go back to reference Canfield, R. L., & Haith, M. M. (1991). Young infants’ visual expectations for symmetric and asymmetric stimulus sequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 198–208.CrossRef Canfield, R. L., & Haith, M. M. (1991). Young infants’ visual expectations for symmetric and asymmetric stimulus sequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 198–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2005). Looking at the rope when looking for the snake: Conceptually mediated eye movements during spoken-word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 453–459. Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2005). Looking at the rope when looking for the snake: Conceptually mediated eye movements during spoken-word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 453–459.
go back to reference Davies, I. R., & Corbett, G. G. (1997). A cross-cultural study of colour grouping: Evidence for weak linguistic relativity. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 493–517.PubMed Davies, I. R., & Corbett, G. G. (1997). A cross-cultural study of colour grouping: Evidence for weak linguistic relativity. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 493–517.PubMed
go back to reference Ganea, P. A., Shutts, K., Spelke, E. S., & DeLoache, J. S. (2007). Thinking of things unseen: Infants’ use of language to update mental representations. Psychological Science, 18, 734–738.CrossRefPubMed Ganea, P. A., Shutts, K., Spelke, E. S., & DeLoache, J. S. (2007). Thinking of things unseen: Infants’ use of language to update mental representations. Psychological Science, 18, 734–738.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gleason, T. R., Fiske, K. E., & Chan, R. K. (2004). The verbal nature of representations of the canonical colors of objects. Cognitive Development, 19, 1–14.CrossRef Gleason, T. R., Fiske, K. E., & Chan, R. K. (2004). The verbal nature of representations of the canonical colors of objects. Cognitive Development, 19, 1–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Graham, S. A., & Diesendruck, G. (2010). Fifteen-month-olds attend to shape over other perceptual properties in an induction task. Cognitive Development, 25, 111–123.CrossRef Graham, S. A., & Diesendruck, G. (2010). Fifteen-month-olds attend to shape over other perceptual properties in an induction task. Cognitive Development, 25, 111–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Graham, S. A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Infants’ reliance on shape to generalize novel labels to animate and inanimate objects. Journal of Child Language, 26, 295–320.CrossRefPubMed Graham, S. A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Infants’ reliance on shape to generalize novel labels to animate and inanimate objects. Journal of Child Language, 26, 295–320.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalitietop model of word learning. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 65 (3, Serial No. 262). Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalitietop model of word learning. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 65 (3, Serial No. 262).
go back to reference Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2004). The online processing of ambiguous and unambiguous words in context: Evidence from head-mounted eye-tracking. In M. Carreiras & C. Clifton (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP and beyond (pp. 187–207). New York: Psychology Press. Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2004). The online processing of ambiguous and unambiguous words in context: Evidence from head-mounted eye-tracking. In M. Carreiras & C. Clifton (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP and beyond (pp. 187–207). New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2005). Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96, B23–B32.CrossRefPubMed Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2005). Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96, B23–B32.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2007). Visual-shape competition during language-mediated attention is based on lexical input and not modulated by contextual appropriateness. Visual Cognition, 15(8), 985–1018.CrossRef Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2007). Visual-shape competition during language-mediated attention is based on lexical input and not modulated by contextual appropriateness. Visual Cognition, 15(8), 985–1018.CrossRef
go back to reference Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2010). Looking at anything that is green when hearing ‘frog’—How object surface color and stored object color knowledge influence language- mediated overt attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (in press). Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2010). Looking at anything that is green when hearing ‘frog’—How object surface color and stored object color knowledge influence language- mediated overt attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Huettig, F., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2008). When you name the pizza you look at the coin and the bread: Eye movements reveal semantic activation during word production. Memory & Cognition, 36, 341–360.CrossRef Huettig, F., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2008). When you name the pizza you look at the coin and the bread: Eye movements reveal semantic activation during word production. Memory & Cognition, 36, 341–360.CrossRef
go back to reference Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2007). The tug of war between phonological, semantic, and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 460–482.CrossRef Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2007). The tug of war between phonological, semantic, and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 460–482.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, E., Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2008). Conceptual attributes of heard words modulate toddlers’ attention to the visual scene. Paper presented at the 11th International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL), Edinburgh, Scotland, UK. Johnson, E., Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2008). Conceptual attributes of heard words modulate toddlers’ attention to the visual scene. Paper presented at the 11th International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL), Edinburgh, Scotland, UK.
go back to reference Johnson, E. K., & Zamuner, T.S. (2010). Using infant and toddler testing methods in language acquisition research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds.), Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins (in press). Johnson, E. K., & Zamuner, T.S. (2010). Using infant and toddler testing methods in language acquisition research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds.), Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins (in press).
go back to reference Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.CrossRef Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.CrossRef
go back to reference Macario, J. F. (1991). Young children’s use of color in classification: foods and canonically colored objects. Cognitive Development, 6, 17–46.CrossRef Macario, J. F. (1991). Young children’s use of color in classification: foods and canonically colored objects. Cognitive Development, 6, 17–46.CrossRef
go back to reference Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic roles. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–27.CrossRef Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic roles. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Meints, K., Plunkett, K., & Harris, P. (1999). When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1072–1078.CrossRefPubMed Meints, K., Plunkett, K., & Harris, P. (1999). When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1072–1078.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Naigles, L. G., & Gelman, S. A. (1995). Overextensions in comprehension and production revisited: Preferential looking study of dog, cat, and cow. Journal of Child Language, 22, 19–46.CrossRefPubMed Naigles, L. G., & Gelman, S. A. (1995). Overextensions in comprehension and production revisited: Preferential looking study of dog, cat, and cow. Journal of Child Language, 22, 19–46.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Odom, R. D., & Guzman, R. D. (1972). Development of hierarchies of dimensional salience. Developmental Psychology, 6, 271–297.CrossRef Odom, R. D., & Guzman, R. D. (1972). Development of hierarchies of dimensional salience. Developmental Psychology, 6, 271–297.CrossRef
go back to reference Saylor, M. (2004). Twelve- and 16-month-old infants recognize properties of mentioned absent things. Developmental Science, 7, 599–611.CrossRefPubMed Saylor, M. (2004). Twelve- and 16-month-old infants recognize properties of mentioned absent things. Developmental Science, 7, 599–611.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Styles, S. J., & Plunkett, K. (2009). How do infants build a semantic system? Language and Cognition, 1, 1–24.CrossRef Styles, S. J., & Plunkett, K. (2009). How do infants build a semantic system? Language and Cognition, 1, 1–24.CrossRef
go back to reference von Koss Torkildsen, J., Syversen, G., Simonsen, H. G., Moen, I., & Lindren, M. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of auditory semantic priming in 24-month-olds. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 332–351.CrossRef von Koss Torkildsen, J., Syversen, G., Simonsen, H. G., Moen, I., & Lindren, M. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of auditory semantic priming in 24-month-olds. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 332–351.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Eye movements during language-mediated visual search reveal a strong link between overt visual attention and lexical processing in 36-month-olds
Auteurs
Elizabeth K. Johnson
Falk Huettig
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0285-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2011

Psychological Research 1/2011 Naar de uitgave

Editorial

Editorial