Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2014

01-03-2014 | Original Article

Exogenous attention can be counter-selective: onset cues disrupt sensitivity to color changes

Auteurs: Gisela Müller-Plath, Nils Klöckner

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In peripheral spatial cueing paradigms, exogenous attentional capture is commonly observed after salient onset cues or with cues contingent on target characteristics. We proposed that exogenously captured attention disrupts the selectivity to target features. We tested this by experimentally emulating the everyday observation that in a viewing situation in which the observer is monitoring a stationary display fort change to occur, the onset of a salient stimulus (onset cue) or a change in a stationary stimulus similar to the expected one (contingent cue) has a distracting effect. As predicted, we found that both types of cues reduced the target detection sensitivity but enhanced the bias to respond in a go-nogo-paradigm. With the onset cue, the sensitivity loss was more pronounced at the side of the cue, whereas the contingent cue affected both sides likewise. Moreover, the effects of the onset cue interacted with the task difficulty: the more selectivity a task required the more immune it was against disruption, but the more likely was a response. We concluded that onset capture disrupts selective attention by adding noise to the processing of the target location. The effects of contingent capture could be explained with cue-target confounding. Finally, we suggest a new model of attentional capture in which exogenous and endogenous components interact in a dynamic way.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The distinction between the mechanisms of channel enhancement and selection is based on a paper by the same first author with the promising title ‘Phenomenology of Attention’ (Prinzmetal, H., Allen, & Edwards, 1998), which, however, might be criticized on several grounds: First, although the majority of experiments in the paper deals with the question of how attention alters the appearance of a color dot, the authors manipulated attention to letters at a location close to the colored dot, and not attention to a specific color or color contrast itself. Had they done this, the appearance of the dot might have changed. Second, they measured appearance by having the subjects match the color of the dot to a color on a test palette. However, by doing this, the subject most plausibly paid attention to the colors of the palette, thereby—if attention altered appearance—it most likely changed the appearance of the test colors as well, thereby canceling out the effect. So it is not surprising that almost no hue shift was observed between ‘attended’ and ‘unattended’ conditions. Third, the colors in the test palette were presented in the context of other colors which might also have changed their appearance (e.g., Ekroll, Faul, & Niederée, 2004).
 
2
Several papers in the literature have shown that the discrimination of weak color differences is improved by and necessitates the allocation of voluntary attention: For example, Corbetta, Miezin, Dobmeyer, Shulman, and Petersen (1991) found that nonspatial attention to color enhanced the accuracy to detect near-threshold changes. Just recently, Jehee, Brady, and Tong (2011) published evidence from a spatial cueing paradigm that voluntary attention to color on either side of the display enhanced the accuracy to detect a near-threshold color change at the cued side (but see Andersen, Müller, & Hillyard, 2009 for the claim that attention to features can also operate location-independently). Nagy, Sanchez, and Hughes (1990) used visual search to demonstrate the effect of voluntary attention on detecting small color differences in foveal as well as peripheral vision.
 
3
Bashinsky and Bacharach (1980) as well as as Müller and Findley (1987) employed a detection plus localization task to circumvent this problem: If a subject responded ‘yes’, he/she was asked to indicate the perceived target position. By relating the cue position to this locational response, the erroneously detected target in a false alarm could be classified as being either validly or invalidly cued. However, beside the question of how these data are analyzed correctly (see Appendix A of Müller & Findley, 1987), in a detection task two conceptual problems arise: First, an objective cue validity (in trials with a target) is confused with a subjective one (in trials without a target), allowing for the possibility that, if a target is erroneously perceived at the nontarget side, the cue is subjectively valid but objectively invalid. Second, it is most likely that performing a detection plus localization task profoundly alters the operation mode of selective attention, compared to detection alone.
Prinzmetal et al. (2008, exp. 3) proceeded differently: At each location, they independently presented either a target or not, and had the subjects judge target presence separately for the two locations. Although this is a nice solution because it allows to assign false alarms to cued and uncued locations, we did not find it suitable for our research question: First, since either 0, 1, or 2 targets could appear, the task is quite complex from the beginning, thus the attentional control set is broader and selectivity less pronounced. Second, target presence at one location might act like an additional 0 ms-SOA-cue for the other one. Third (although implicitly) the task is detection plus localization again. Finally, the procedure is not suited to test the alertness hypothesis, i.e., the effect of the cue on the bias to respond.
Liu et al. (2005) used a so-called response cue at the end of each trial, telling the subject which stimulus position to respond to. Besides this procedure seeming more elegant than the double response of Müller and Findley (1987), it poses the same problems when applied to a detection task. Moreover, it is not suitable for investigating the alertness hypothesis again.
 
4
Note that this is different from the ROC plots in Fig. 2 where the SDT model had been fitted to the pooled data of all subjects. Generally, the SDT with averaged parameter estimates does not provide a good fit to the pooled data. Figure 4 should illustrate the hypotheses tests, which are carried out on the mean parameter estimates, whereas Fig. 2 should illustrate the model fit. Note further that all model fits were carried out with the constraint of equidistant criteria λlib, λbal, and λcon for cued and uncued trials.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, D. A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological consideration of attention and action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Allport, D. A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological consideration of attention and action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Andersen, S. K., Müller, M. M., Hillyard, S. A. (2009). Color-selective attention need not be mediated by spatial attention. Journal of Vision, 9(6), 1–7.PubMedCrossRef Andersen, S. K., Müller, M. M., Hillyard, S. A. (2009). Color-selective attention need not be mediated by spatial attention. Journal of Vision, 9(6), 1–7.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29(5), 937–948. Ansorge, U., Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29(5), 937–948.
go back to reference Awh, E., Belopolsky, A., Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437–443.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Awh, E., Belopolsky, A., Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437–443.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bashinsky, H. S., Bacharach, V. R. (1980). Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations. Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 241–248.CrossRef Bashinsky, H. S., Bacharach, V. R. (1980). Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations. Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 241–248.CrossRef
go back to reference Benjamin, A. S., Diaz, M. L., Wee, S. (2009). Signal detection with criterion noise: Applications to recognition memory. Psychological Review, 116, 84–114.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Benjamin, A. S., Diaz, M. L., Wee, S. (2009). Signal detection with criterion noise: Applications to recognition memory. Psychological Review, 116, 84–114.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beringer, J. (1987). Experimental Run Time System (ERTS): Software for developing and running reaction time experiments on IBM-compatibe PCs (Version v3.31). Frankfurt/M: BeriSoft Cooperation. Beringer, J. (1987). Experimental Run Time System (ERTS): Software for developing and running reaction time experiments on IBM-compatibe PCs (Version v3.31). Frankfurt/M: BeriSoft Cooperation.
go back to reference Bonnel, A. M., Miller, J (1994). Attentional effects on concurrent discriminations: Investigations of a sample-size model. Perception and Psychophysics, 54, 162–179.CrossRef Bonnel, A. M., Miller, J (1994). Attentional effects on concurrent discriminations: Investigations of a sample-size model. Perception and Psychophysics, 54, 162–179.CrossRef
go back to reference Bradley, J. (1968). Distribution-free statistical tests. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. Bradley, J. (1968). Distribution-free statistical tests. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Carrasco, M., Ling, S., Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 308–313.PubMedCrossRef Carrasco, M., Ling, S., Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 308–313.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carrasco, M., Penpeci-Talgar, C., Eckstein, M. (2000). Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement. Vision Research, 40, 1203–1215.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Carrasco, M., Penpeci-Talgar, C., Eckstein, M. (2000). Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement. Vision Research, 40, 1203–1215.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Castiello, U., Umiltà, C. (1990). Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of processing. Acta Psychologica, 73, 195–209.PubMedCrossRef Castiello, U., Umiltà, C. (1990). Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of processing. Acta Psychologica, 73, 195–209.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chastain, G. (1992). Is rapid performance improvement across short precue-target delays due to masking from peripheral precues? Acta Psychologica, 79, 101–114.PubMedCrossRef Chastain, G. (1992). Is rapid performance improvement across short precue-target delays due to masking from peripheral precues? Acta Psychologica, 79, 101–114.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G. L., Petersen, S. E. (1991). Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. The Journal of Neuroscience, 11(8), 2383–2401.PubMed Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G. L., Petersen, S. E. (1991). Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. The Journal of Neuroscience, 11(8), 2383–2401.PubMed
go back to reference Cosman, J. D., Vecera, S. P. (2009). Perceptual load modulates attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 404–410.CrossRef Cosman, J. D., Vecera, S. P. (2009). Perceptual load modulates attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 404–410.CrossRef
go back to reference Danilova, M. V., Mollon, J. D. (2011). Parafoveal color discrimination: a chromaticity locus of enhanced discrimination. Journal of Vision, 10(1), 1–9.CrossRef Danilova, M. V., Mollon, J. D. (2011). Parafoveal color discrimination: a chromaticity locus of enhanced discrimination. Journal of Vision, 10(1), 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Eisner, A., MacLeod, D. I. A. (1980). Blue-sensitive cones do not contribute to luminance. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 70(1), 121–123.PubMedCrossRef Eisner, A., MacLeod, D. I. A. (1980). Blue-sensitive cones do not contribute to luminance. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 70(1), 121–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ekroll, V., Faul, F., Niederée, R. (2004). The peculiar nature of simultaneous colour contrast in uniform surrounds. Vision Research, 44, 1765–1786.PubMedCrossRef Ekroll, V., Faul, F., Niederée, R. (2004). The peculiar nature of simultaneous colour contrast in uniform surrounds. Vision Research, 44, 1765–1786.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W., St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(4), 225–240.CrossRef Eriksen, C. W., St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(4), 225–240.CrossRef
go back to reference Facoetti, A., Corradi, N., Ruffino, M., Gori, S., Zorzi, M. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 22(5), 1011–1025. Facoetti, A., Corradi, N., Ruffino, M., Gori, S., Zorzi, M. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 22(5), 1011–1025.
go back to reference Facoetti, A., Molteni, M. (2001). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 39, 352–357.PubMedCrossRef Facoetti, A., Molteni, M. (2001). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 39, 352–357.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., Zorzi, M. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1011–1025.PubMedCrossRef Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., Zorzi, M. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1011–1025.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030–1044.PubMed Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030–1044.PubMed
go back to reference Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 1–6.CrossRef Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 1–6.CrossRef
go back to reference Fuller, S., Carrasco, M. (2006). Exogenous attention and color perception: Performance and appearance of saturation and hue. Vision Research, 46, 4032–4047.PubMedCrossRef Fuller, S., Carrasco, M. (2006). Exogenous attention and color perception: Performance and appearance of saturation and hue. Vision Research, 46, 4032–4047.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilbert, M. (1950). Colour perception in parafoveal vision. Proceedings of the Physical Society London, 63, 83–89.CrossRef Gilbert, M. (1950). Colour perception in parafoveal vision. Proceedings of the Physical Society London, 63, 83–89.CrossRef
go back to reference Gould, I. C., Wolfgang, B. W., Smith, P. L. (2007). Spatial uncertainty explains exogenous and endogenous attentional cuing effects in visual signal detection. Journal of Vision, 7(4), 1–17.PubMedCrossRef Gould, I. C., Wolfgang, B. W., Smith, P. L. (2007). Spatial uncertainty explains exogenous and endogenous attentional cuing effects in visual signal detection. Journal of Vision, 7(4), 1–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Green, D. M., Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York: Wiley. Green, D. M., Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Henderson, J. M. (1991). Stimulus discrimination following covert attentional orienting to an exogenous cue. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 91–106.PubMed Henderson, J. M. (1991). Stimulus discrimination following covert attentional orienting to an exogenous cue. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 91–106.PubMed
go back to reference Henderson, J. M., Macquistan, A. (1993). The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue. Perception and Psychophysics, 53(2), 221–230.PubMedCrossRef Henderson, J. M., Macquistan, A. (1993). The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue. Perception and Psychophysics, 53(2), 221–230.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Dover (Original work published 1890). James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Dover (Original work published 1890).
go back to reference Jehee, J. F. M., Brady, D. K., Tong, F. (2011). Attention improves encoding of task-relevant features in the human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(22), 8210–8219.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Jehee, J. F. M., Brady, D. K., Tong, F. (2011). Attention improves encoding of task-relevant features in the human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(22), 8210–8219.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary vs. automatic control of the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–204). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary vs. automatic control of the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–204). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Kerzel, D., Zarian, L., Suoto, D. (2009). Involuntary cueing effects on accuracy measures: Stimulus and task dependence. Journal of Vision, 9(11), 1–16.CrossRef Kerzel, D., Zarian, L., Suoto, D. (2009). Involuntary cueing effects on accuracy measures: Stimulus and task dependence. Journal of Vision, 9(11), 1–16.CrossRef
go back to reference LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371–379.PubMed LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371–379.PubMed
go back to reference LaBerge, D., Brown, V. (1986). Variations in size of the visual field in which targets are presented: An attentional range effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(3), 188–200.CrossRef LaBerge, D., Brown, V. (1986). Variations in size of the visual field in which targets are presented: An attentional range effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(3), 188–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Lambert, A., Hockey, R. (1991). Peripheral visual changes and spatial attention. Acta Psychologica, 76, 149–163.PubMedCrossRef Lambert, A., Hockey, R. (1991). Peripheral visual changes and spatial attention. Acta Psychologica, 76, 149–163.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468.PubMed Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468.PubMed
go back to reference Lehmann, E. L., Stein, C. (1949). On the theory of some non-parametric hypotheses. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 20(1), 28–45.CrossRef Lehmann, E. L., Stein, C. (1949). On the theory of some non-parametric hypotheses. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 20(1), 28–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Liu, T., Pestilli, F., Carrasco, M. (2005). Transient attention enhances perceptual performance and fMRI response in human visual cortex. Neuron, 45, 469–477.PubMedCrossRef Liu, T., Pestilli, F., Carrasco, M. (2005). Transient attention enhances perceptual performance and fMRI response in human visual cortex. Neuron, 45, 469–477.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lu, Z.-L., & Dosher, B. (2005). Neurobiology of attention. In L. Itti, G. Rees, & J. Tsotsos (Eds.), (pp. 448–453). New York: Academic Press. Lu, Z.-L., & Dosher, B. (2005). Neurobiology of attention. In L. Itti, G. Rees, & J. Tsotsos (Eds.), (pp. 448–453). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Lu, Z.-L., Dosher, B. A. (1998). External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vision Research, 38, 1183–1198.PubMedCrossRef Lu, Z.-L., Dosher, B. A. (1998). External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vision Research, 38, 1183–1198.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Luck, S. J., Thomas, S. J. (1999). What variety of attention is automatically captured by peripheral cues? Perception and Psychophysics, 61(7), 1424–1435.PubMedCrossRef Luck, S. J., Thomas, S. J. (1999). What variety of attention is automatically captured by peripheral cues? Perception and Psychophysics, 61(7), 1424–1435.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Marcum, J. I. (1947). A statistical theory of target detection by pulsed radar (Research Memorandum RM- 754) (Technical Report). Project RAND, Canada: Douglas Aircraft Company, Inc. Marcum, J. I. (1947). A statistical theory of target detection by pulsed radar (Research Memorandum RM- 754) (Technical Report). Project RAND, Canada: Douglas Aircraft Company, Inc.
go back to reference Mueller, S. T., Weidemann, C. T. (2008). Decision noise: An explanation for observed violations of signal detection theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 465–494.CrossRef Mueller, S. T., Weidemann, C. T. (2008). Decision noise: An explanation for observed violations of signal detection theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 465–494.CrossRef
go back to reference Müller, H. J., Findley, J. M. (1987). Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 383–399.CrossRef Müller, H. J., Findley, J. M. (1987). Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 383–399.CrossRef
go back to reference Müller, H. J., Rabbit, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and Voluntary Orienting of Visual Attention: Time Course of Activation and Resistance to Interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 315–330.PubMed Müller, H. J., Rabbit, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and Voluntary Orienting of Visual Attention: Time Course of Activation and Resistance to Interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 315–330.PubMed
go back to reference Müller, N. G., Mollenhauer, M., Rösler, A., Kleinschmidt, A. (2005). The attentional field has a mexican hat distribution. Vision Research, 45, 1129–1137.PubMedCrossRef Müller, N. G., Mollenhauer, M., Rösler, A., Kleinschmidt, A. (2005). The attentional field has a mexican hat distribution. Vision Research, 45, 1129–1137.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Müller-Plath, G. (2008). Localizing subprocesses of visual search by correlating local brain activation in fMRI with response time model parameters. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 171(2), 316–330.PubMedCrossRef Müller-Plath, G. (2008). Localizing subprocesses of visual search by correlating local brain activation in fMRI with response time model parameters. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 171(2), 316–330.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nagy, A. L., Sanchez, R. R., Hughes, T. C. (1990). Visual search for color differences with foveal and peripheral vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 7(10), 1995–2001.CrossRef Nagy, A. L., Sanchez, R. R., Hughes, T. C. (1990). Visual search for color differences with foveal and peripheral vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 7(10), 1995–2001.CrossRef
go back to reference Neumann, O. (1987). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on Perception and Action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Neumann, O. (1987). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on Perception and Action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Ossandón, J. P., Onat, S., Cazzoli, D., Nyffeler, T., Müri, R., König, P. (2012). Unmasking the contribution of low-level features to the guidance of attention. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3478–3487.PubMedCrossRef Ossandón, J. P., Onat, S., Cazzoli, D., Nyffeler, T., Müri, R., König, P. (2012). Unmasking the contribution of low-level features to the guidance of attention. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3478–3487.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pestilli, F., Carrasco, M. (2005). Attention enhances contrast sensitivity at cued and impairs it at uncued locations. Vision Research, 45, 1867–1875.PubMedCrossRef Pestilli, F., Carrasco, M. (2005). Attention enhances contrast sensitivity at cued and impairs it at uncued locations. Vision Research, 45, 1867–1875.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRef Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance X. Hove, UK: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance X. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Prinzmetal, W., H., A., Allen, K., & Edwards, T. (1998). Phenomenology of attention: 1. color, location, orientation, and spatial frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 261-282. Prinzmetal, W., H., A., Allen, K., & Edwards, T. (1998). Phenomenology of attention: 1. color, location, orientation, and spatial frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 261-282.
go back to reference Prinzmetal, W., Long, V., Leonhardt, J. (2008). Involuntary attention and brightness contrast. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(7), 1139–1150.CrossRef Prinzmetal, W., Long, V., Leonhardt, J. (2008). Involuntary attention and brightness contrast. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(7), 1139–1150.CrossRef
go back to reference Prinzmetal, W., McCool, C., Park, S. (2005). Attention: reaction time and accuracy reveal different mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 73–92.CrossRef Prinzmetal, W., McCool, C., Park, S. (2005). Attention: reaction time and accuracy reveal different mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 73–92.CrossRef
go back to reference Prinzmetal, W., Zvinyatskovskiy, A., Gutierrez, P., Dilem, L. (2009). Voluntary and involuntary attention have different consequences: The effect of perceptual difficulty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 352–369.PubMedCrossRef Prinzmetal, W., Zvinyatskovskiy, A., Gutierrez, P., Dilem, L. (2009). Voluntary and involuntary attention have different consequences: The effect of perceptual difficulty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 352–369.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reynolds, J. H., Pasternak, T., Desimone, R. (2000). Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron, 26, 703–714.PubMedCrossRef Reynolds, J. H., Pasternak, T., Desimone, R. (2000). Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron, 26, 703–714.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ronconi, L., Basso, D., Gori, S., & Facoetti, A. (2013a). TMS on right frontal eye fields induces an inflexible focus of attention. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs319 Ronconi, L., Basso, D., Gori, S., & Facoetti, A. (2013a). TMS on right frontal eye fields induces an inflexible focus of attention. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhs319
go back to reference Schneider, K. A. (2006). Does attention alter appearance? Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 800–814.CrossRef Schneider, K. A. (2006). Does attention alter appearance? Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 800–814.CrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, K. A., Komlos, M. (2008). Attention biases decisions but does not alter appearance. Journal of Vision, 8(15), 1–10.CrossRef Schneider, K. A., Komlos, M. (2008). Attention biases decisions but does not alter appearance. Journal of Vision, 8(15), 1–10.CrossRef
go back to reference Schreij, D., Owens, C., Theeuwes, J. (2008). Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(2), 208–218.CrossRef Schreij, D., Owens, C., Theeuwes, J. (2008). Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(2), 208–218.CrossRef
go back to reference Shiu, L., Pashler, H. (1994). Negligible effect of spatial precuing on identification of single digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1037–1054. Shiu, L., Pashler, H. (1994). Negligible effect of spatial precuing on identification of single digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1037–1054.
go back to reference Styles, E. A. (2006). The Psychology of Attention. Oxford: Psychology Press. Styles, E. A. (2006). The Psychology of Attention. Oxford: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Swets, J. A. (1996). Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis in Psychology and Diagnostics. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Swets, J. A. (1996). Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis in Psychology and Diagnostics. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Tassinari, G., Aglioti, S., Chelazzi, L., Peru, A., Berlucchi, G. (1994). Do peripheral non-informative cues induce early facilitation of target detection? Vision Research, 34(2), 197–189.CrossRef Tassinari, G., Aglioti, S., Chelazzi, L., Peru, A., Berlucchi, G. (1994). Do peripheral non-informative cues induce early facilitation of target detection? Vision Research, 34(2), 197–189.CrossRef
go back to reference Tassinari, G., Berlucchi, G. (1993). Sensory and attentional components of slowing of manual reaction time to non-fixated visual targets by ipsilateral primes. Vision Research, 33(11), 1525–1534.PubMedCrossRef Tassinari, G., Berlucchi, G. (1993). Sensory and attentional components of slowing of manual reaction time to non-fixated visual targets by ipsilateral primes. Vision Research, 33(11), 1525–1534.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1991). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 184–193.CrossRef Theeuwes, J. (1991). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 184–193.CrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. M. (1998). Feature binding, attention and object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B, 353, 1295–1306.CrossRef Treisman, A. M. (1998). Feature binding, attention and object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B, 353, 1295–1306.CrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A. M., Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tsushima, Y., Sasaki, Y., Watanabe, T. (2006). Greater disruption due to failure of inhibitory control on an ambiguous distractor. Science, 314(5806), 1786–1788. Tsushima, Y., Sasaki, Y., Watanabe, T. (2006). Greater disruption due to failure of inhibitory control on an ambiguous distractor. Science, 314(5806), 1786–1788.
go back to reference Wei, P., Müller, H. J., Pollmann, S., Zhou, X. (2011). Neural correlates of binding features within-or crossdimensions in visual conjunction search: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 57, 235–241.PubMedCrossRef Wei, P., Müller, H. J., Pollmann, S., Zhou, X. (2011). Neural correlates of binding features within-or crossdimensions in visual conjunction search: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 57, 235–241.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wickens, T. D. (2002). Elementary signal detection theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Wickens, T. D. (2002). Elementary signal detection theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Wienrich, C., Janczyk, M. (2011) Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73, 2044–2052.CrossRef Wienrich, C., Janczyk, M. (2011) Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73, 2044–2052.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.CrossRef Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Wundt, W. (1880). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie [Principles of Physiological psychology] (2nd ed., Vol. 2). Leipzig: Engelmann. Wundt, W. (1880). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie [Principles of Physiological psychology] (2nd ed., Vol. 2). Leipzig: Engelmann.
go back to reference Yantis, S., Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.PubMed Yantis, S., Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Exogenous attention can be counter-selective: onset cues disrupt sensitivity to color changes
Auteurs
Gisela Müller-Plath
Nils Klöckner
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0489-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2014

Psychological Research 2/2014 Naar de uitgave