Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2015

14-11-2014 | Original Article

Embodied markedness of parity? Examining handedness effects on parity judgments

Auteurs: Stefan Huber, Elise Klein, Martina Graf, Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Korbinian Moeller, Klaus Willmes

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Parity is important semantic information encoded by numbers. Interestingly, there are hand-based effects in parity judgment tasks: right-hand responses are faster for even and left-hand responses for odd numbers. As this effect was initially explained by the markedness of the words even vs. odd and right vs. left, it was denoted as the linguistic markedness of response codes (MARC) effect. In the present study, we investigated whether the MARC effect differs for right and left handers. We conducted a parity judgment task, in which right- and left-handed participants had to decide whether a presented single or two-digit number was odd or even by pressing a corresponding response key. We found that handedness modulated the MARC effect for unit digits. While we replicated a regular MARC effect for right handers, there was no evidence for a MARC effect for left handers. However, closer inspection revealed that the MARC effect in left handers depended on the degree of left-handedness with a reversed MARC effect for most left-handed participants. Furthermore, although parity of tens digits interfered with the processing of unit digits, the MARC effect for tens digits was not modulated by handedness. Our findings are discussed in the light of three different accounts for the MARC effect: the linguistic markedness account, the polarity correspondence principle, and the body-specificity hypothesis.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
In parity judgment tasks also an odd effect can be observed with faster responses for even than for odd numbers (Hines, 1990).
 
2
Recently, Tan and Dixon (2011) reported a response repetition effect in a parity judgment task. Independent of the aims of the present study, we also tested whether we were able to replicate these findings. The results of the replication are given in Appendix B.
 
3
We wish to thank an anonymous reviewer for the suggestion of including gender as a predictor variable.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.CrossRef Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.CrossRef
go back to reference Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.CrossRef Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.CrossRef
go back to reference Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4), 286–308. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2518.CrossRefPubMed Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4), 286–308. doi:10.​1006/​jecp.​1999.​2518.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Beringer, J. (1994). ERTS: a flexible software tool for developing and running psychological reaction time experiments on IBM PCs. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 26(3), 368–369. doi:10.3758/BF03204646.CrossRef Beringer, J. (1994). ERTS: a flexible software tool for developing and running psychological reaction time experiments on IBM PCs. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 26(3), 368–369. doi:10.​3758/​BF03204646.CrossRef
go back to reference Bull, R., Cleland, A. A., & Mitchell, T. (2013). Sex differences in the spatial representation of number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 181–192. doi:10.1037/a0028387.CrossRef Bull, R., Cleland, A. A., & Mitchell, T. (2013). Sex differences in the spatial representation of number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 181–192. doi:10.​1037/​a0028387.CrossRef
go back to reference Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351–367. doi:10.1037/a0015854.CrossRef Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351–367. doi:10.​1037/​a0015854.CrossRef
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2007). When is an odd number not odd? Influence of task rule on the MARC effect for numeric classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(5), 832–842. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.832.PubMed Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2007). When is an odd number not odd? Influence of task rule on the MARC effect for numeric classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(5), 832–842. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​33.​5.​832.PubMed
go back to reference de la Vega, I., Dudschig, C., De Filippis, M., Lachmair, M., & Kaup, B. (2013). Keep your hands crossed: The valence-by-left/right interaction is related to hand, not side, in an incongruent hand–response key assignment. Acta Psychologica, 142(2), 273–277. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.12.011.CrossRefPubMed de la Vega, I., Dudschig, C., De Filippis, M., Lachmair, M., & Kaup, B. (2013). Keep your hands crossed: The valence-by-left/right interaction is related to hand, not side, in an incongruent hand–response key assignment. Acta Psychologica, 142(2), 273–277. doi:10.​1016/​j.​actpsy.​2012.​12.​011.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626–641.PubMed Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626–641.PubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial-Numerical Associations Point To Finger Counting as Prime Example of Embodied Cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 260.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial-Numerical Associations Point To Finger Counting as Prime Example of Embodied Cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 260.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression (2nd ed.). Sage: Thousand Oaks. Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression (2nd ed.). Sage: Thousand Oaks.
go back to reference Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2010). Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 180–190. doi:10.1037/a0017688.CrossRef Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2010). Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 180–190. doi:10.​1037/​a0017688.CrossRef
go back to reference Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65-70. Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65-70.
go back to reference Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 346–363.CrossRefPubMed Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 346–363.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huber, S., Moeller, K., Nuerk, H. C., Macizo, P., Herrera, A., & Willmes, K. (2013). Cognitive control in number processing—A computational model. In: R. West & T. Stewart (Eds.), Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (pp. 185–190). Ottawa: Carleton University. Huber, S., Moeller, K., Nuerk, H. C., Macizo, P., Herrera, A., & Willmes, K. (2013). Cognitive control in number processing—A computational model. In: R. West & T. Stewart (Eds.), Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (pp. 185–190). Ottawa: Carleton University.
go back to reference Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32(4), 662–673.CrossRefPubMed Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32(4), 662–673.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kilshaw, D., & Annett, M. (1983). Right- and left-hand skill I: effects of age, sex and hand preference showing superior skill in left-handers. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 253–268.CrossRefPubMed Kilshaw, D., & Annett, M. (1983). Right- and left-hand skill I: effects of age, sex and hand preference showing superior skill in left-handers. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 253–268.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kinoshita, S., & Peek-O’Leary, M. (2006). Two bases of the compatibility effect in the Implicit Association Test (IAT). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(12), 2020–2120. doi:10.1080/17470210500451141. Kinoshita, S., & Peek-O’Leary, M. (2006). Two bases of the compatibility effect in the Implicit Association Test (IAT). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(12), 2020–2120. doi:10.​1080/​1747021050045114​1.
go back to reference Lindemann, O., Abolafia, J. M., Pratt, J., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Coding strategies in number space: memory requirements influence spatial-numerical associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(4), 515–524. doi:10.1080/17470210701728677.CrossRef Lindemann, O., Abolafia, J. M., Pratt, J., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Coding strategies in number space: memory requirements influence spatial-numerical associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(4), 515–524. doi:10.​1080/​1747021070172867​7.CrossRef
go back to reference Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger counting habits in middle eastern and western individuals: an online survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 566–578. doi:10.1177/0022022111406254.CrossRef Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger counting habits in middle eastern and western individuals: an online survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 566–578. doi:10.​1177/​0022022111406254​.CrossRef
go back to reference Lorch, R. F, Jr, & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(1), 149–157.PubMed Lorch, R. F, Jr, & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(1), 149–157.PubMed
go back to reference Moeller, K., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2011). Two-digit number processing: holistic, decomposed or hybrid? A computational modelling approach. Psychological Research, 75(4), 290–306.PubMed Moeller, K., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2011). Two-digit number processing: holistic, decomposed or hybrid? A computational modelling approach. Psychological Research, 75(4), 290–306.PubMed
go back to reference Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., Klein, E., Willmes, K., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line: a review of multi-digit number processing. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 3–22.CrossRef Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., Klein, E., Willmes, K., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line: a review of multi-digit number processing. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 3–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Affective Judgments. Psychological Science, 9(1), 45–48.CrossRef Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Affective Judgments. Psychological Science, 9(1), 45–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(4), 651–664. doi:10.3758/BF03194909.CrossRefPubMed Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(4), 651–664. doi:10.​3758/​BF03194909.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shaki, S., & Petrusic, W. M. (2005). On the mental representation of negative numbers: context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 931–937.CrossRefPubMed Shaki, S., & Petrusic, W. M. (2005). On the mental representation of negative numbers: context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 931–937.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shaki, S., Petrusic, W. M., & Leth-Steensen, C. (2012). SNARC effects with numerical and non-numerical symbolic comparative judgments: instructional and cultural dependencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 515–530. doi:10.1037/a0026729.PubMed Shaki, S., Petrusic, W. M., & Leth-Steensen, C. (2012). SNARC effects with numerical and non-numerical symbolic comparative judgments: instructional and cultural dependencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 515–530. doi:10.​1037/​a0026729.PubMed
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.CrossRef Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.CrossRef
go back to reference Willmes, K., & Iversen, W. (1995). On the internal representation of number parity. Paper presented at the Spring Annual General Meeting of the British Neuropsychological Society. London. Willmes, K., & Iversen, W. (1995). On the internal representation of number parity. Paper presented at the Spring Annual General Meeting of the British Neuropsychological Society. London.
go back to reference Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(4), 489–525. Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(4), 489–525.
go back to reference Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5(1–2), 1–2. Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5(1–2), 1–2.
go back to reference Zimmer, K. E. (1964). Affixed negation in English and other languages: An investigation of restricted productivity. Word, 20, 2, Monograph No. 5. Zimmer, K. E. (1964). Affixed negation in English and other languages: An investigation of restricted productivity. Word, 20, 2, Monograph No. 5.
Metagegevens
Titel
Embodied markedness of parity? Examining handedness effects on parity judgments
Auteurs
Stefan Huber
Elise Klein
Martina Graf
Hans-Christoph Nuerk
Korbinian Moeller
Klaus Willmes
Publicatiedatum
14-11-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0626-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2015

Psychological Research 6/2015 Naar de uitgave