Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1/2013

01-01-2013

Effortful Control in “Hot” and “Cool” Tasks Differentially Predicts Children’s Behavior Problems and Academic Performance

Auteurs: Sanghag Kim, Jamie Koenig Nordling, Jeung Eun Yoon, Lea J. Boldt, Grazyna Kochanska

Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology | Uitgave 1/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Effortful control (EC), the capacity to deliberately suppress a dominant response and perform a subdominant response, rapidly developing in toddler and preschool age, has been shown to be a robust predictor of children’s adjustment. Not settled, however, is whether a view of EC as a heterogeneous rather than unidimensional construct may offer advantages in the context of predicting diverse developmental outcomes. This study focused on the potential distinction between “hot” EC function (delay-of-gratification tasks that called for suppressing an emotionally charged response) and more abstract “cool” EC functions (motor inhibition tasks, suppressing-initiating response or Go-No Go tasks, and effortful attention or Stroop-like tasks). Children (N = 100) were observed performing EC tasks at 38 and 52 months. Mothers, fathers, and teachers rated children’s behavior problems and academic performance at 67, 80, and 100 months, and children participated in a clinical interview at 100 months. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analyses with latent variables produced consistent findings across all informants: Children’s scores in “hot” EC tasks, presumably engaging emotion regulation skills, predicted behavior problems but not academic performance, whereas their scores in “cool” EC tasks, specifically those engaging effortful attention, predicted academic performance but not behavior problems. The models of EC as a heterogeneous construct offered some advantages over the unidimensional models. Methodological and clinical implications of the findings are discussed.
Literatuur
go back to reference Allan, N. P., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Examining the dimensionality of effortful control in preschool children and its relation to academic and socioemotional indicators. Developmental Psychology, 47, 905–915.PubMedCrossRef Allan, N. P., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Examining the dimensionality of effortful control in preschool children and its relation to academic and socioemotional indicators. Developmental Psychology, 47, 905–915.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). (DSM-IV-TR) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.. text revision. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). (DSM-IV-TR) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.. text revision.
go back to reference Arseneault, L., Kim-Cohen, J., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2005). Psychometric evaluation of 5-and 7-year-old children’s self-reports of conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 537–550.PubMedCrossRef Arseneault, L., Kim-Cohen, J., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2005). Psychometric evaluation of 5-and 7-year-old children’s self-reports of conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 537–550.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.PubMedCrossRef Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.PubMedCrossRef Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.PubMedCrossRef Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663.PubMedCrossRef Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley. Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Boyce, W. T., Essex, M. J., Woodward, H. R., Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. I: exploring the “headwaters” of early life morbidities. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 580–587.PubMedCrossRef Boyce, W. T., Essex, M. J., Woodward, H. R., Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. I: exploring the “headwaters” of early life morbidities. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 580–587.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Breton, J. J., Bergeron, L., Valla, J. P., Berthiaume, C., & Gaudet, N. (1999). Quebec Child Mental Health Survey: prevalence of DSM-III-R mental health disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 375–384.PubMedCrossRef Breton, J. J., Bergeron, L., Valla, J. P., Berthiaume, C., & Gaudet, N. (1999). Quebec Child Mental Health Survey: prevalence of DSM-III-R mental health disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 375–384.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 337–349.CrossRef Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 337–349.CrossRef
go back to reference Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulated cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4–6, 215–222.CrossRef Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulated cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4–6, 215–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: a multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 477–498.PubMedCrossRef Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: a multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 477–498.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.PubMedCrossRef Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 300–365). New York: Wiley. Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 300–365). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Cole, P. M., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2009). Emotion regulation, risk, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1327–1330.PubMedCrossRef Cole, P. M., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2009). Emotion regulation, risk, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1327–1330.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Smith, K. D. (1994). Expressive control during a disappointment: variations related to preschoolers’ behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 30, 835–846.CrossRef Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Smith, K. D. (1994). Expressive control during a disappointment: variations related to preschoolers’ behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 30, 835–846.CrossRef
go back to reference Cole, P. M., Luby, J., & Sullivan, M. W. (2008). Emotions and the development of childhood depression: bridging the gap. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 141–148.PubMedCrossRef Cole, P. M., Luby, J., & Sullivan, M. W. (2008). Emotions and the development of childhood depression: bridging the gap. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 141–148.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Denham, S. A., Warren-Khot, H. K., Bassett, H. H., Wyatt, T., & Perna, A. (2012). Factor structure of self-regulation in preschoolers: testing models of a field-based assessment for predicting early school readiness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 386–404.PubMedCrossRef Denham, S. A., Warren-Khot, H. K., Bassett, H. H., Wyatt, T., & Perna, A. (2012). Factor structure of self-regulation in preschoolers: testing models of a field-based assessment for predicting early school readiness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 386–404.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9(04), 633–652.PubMedCrossRef Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9(04), 633–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 25–268.CrossRef Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 25–268.CrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., Shepard, S. A., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Cumberland, A. J. (2003). The relations of EC and ego-control to children’s resiliency and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 761–776.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., Shepard, S. A., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Cumberland, A. J. (2003). The relations of EC and ego-control to children’s resiliency and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 761–776.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Champion, C., & Ma, Y. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: an emerging construct. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 236–259.CrossRef Eisenberg, N., Champion, C., & Ma, Y. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: an emerging construct. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 236–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., Valiente, C., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., & Thompson, M. (2004). The relations of EC and impulsivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75(1), 25–46.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., Valiente, C., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., & Thompson, M. (2004). The relations of EC and impulsivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75(1), 25–46.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A. C., & Moehr, M. J. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 33–46.PubMedCrossRef Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A. C., & Moehr, M. J. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 33–46.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 588–603.PubMedCrossRef Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 588–603.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 55–68. Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 55–68.
go back to reference Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (2002). Child Symptom Inventory-4: Screening and norms manual. Stony Brook: Checkmate Plus. Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (2002). Child Symptom Inventory-4: Screening and norms manual. Stony Brook: Checkmate Plus.
go back to reference Gadow, K. D., Sprafkin, J., & Nolan, E. E. (2001). DSM-IV Symptoms in community and clinic preschool children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1383–1392.PubMedCrossRef Gadow, K. D., Sprafkin, J., & Nolan, E. E. (2001). DSM-IV Symptoms in community and clinic preschool children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1383–1392.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gusdorf, L. M. A., Karreman, A., van Aken, M. A. G., Dekovic, M., & van Tuijl, C. (2011). The structure of effortful control in preschoolers and its relation to externalizing problems. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 612–634.PubMedCrossRef Gusdorf, L. M. A., Karreman, A., van Aken, M. A. G., Dekovic, M., & van Tuijl, C. (2011). The structure of effortful control in preschoolers and its relation to externalizing problems. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 612–634.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644.PubMedCrossRef Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.CrossRef Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.CrossRef
go back to reference Huijbregts, S. C. J., Warren, A. J., de Sonneville, L. M. J., & Swaab-Barneveld, H. (2008). Hot and cool forms of inhibitory control and externalizing behavior in children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: an exploratory study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(3), 323–333.PubMedCrossRef Huijbregts, S. C. J., Warren, A. J., de Sonneville, L. M. J., & Swaab-Barneveld, H. (2008). Hot and cool forms of inhibitory control and externalizing behavior in children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: an exploratory study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(3), 323–333.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 418–434.CrossRef Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 418–434.CrossRef
go back to reference Keenan, K., Hipwell, A. E., Hinze, A. E., & Babinski, D. E. (2009). The association of pain and depression in preadolescent girls: moderation by race and pubertal stage. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 727–737.PubMedCrossRef Keenan, K., Hipwell, A. E., Hinze, A. E., & Babinski, D. E. (2009). The association of pain and depression in preadolescent girls: moderation by race and pubertal stage. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 727–737.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). EC as a personality characteristic of young children: antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). EC as a personality characteristic of young children: antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: from toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68, 263–277.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: from toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68, 263–277.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Jimenez, N. B., Hollatz, A. L., & Woodard, J. (2009). Guilt and effortful control: two mechanisms that prevent disruptive developmental trajectories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 322–333.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Jimenez, N. B., Hollatz, A. L., & Woodard, J. (2009). Guilt and effortful control: two mechanisms that prevent disruptive developmental trajectories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 322–333.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: a developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.CrossRef Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: a developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.CrossRef
go back to reference Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., White, J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Delay of gratification, psychopathology, and personality: is low self-control specific to externalizing problems? Journal of Personality, 64, 107–129.PubMedCrossRef Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., White, J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Delay of gratification, psychopathology, and personality: is low self-control specific to externalizing problems? Journal of Personality, 64, 107–129.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 677–692.PubMedCrossRef Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 677–692.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marsh, H. W., & Hocevar, D. (1988). A new, more powerful approach to multitrait-multimethod analyses: application of second-order confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(1), 107–117.CrossRef Marsh, H. W., & Hocevar, D. (1988). A new, more powerful approach to multitrait-multimethod analyses: application of second-order confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(1), 107–117.CrossRef
go back to reference Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.PubMedCrossRef Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 271–289.PubMedCrossRef Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 271–289.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395–422.PubMedCrossRef Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395–422.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427–441.CrossRef Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Temperament and self- regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self- regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 357–370). New York: Guilford Press. Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Temperament and self- regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self- regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 357–370). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Rueda, C. (In press). Effortful control. In M. Zentner & R. Shiner (Eds.), The Handbook of Temperament. New York: Guilford Press. Rueda, C. (In press). Effortful control. In M. Zentner & R. Shiner (Eds.), The Handbook of Temperament. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.CrossRef Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.CrossRef
go back to reference Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Dalen, L., Daley, D., & Remington, B. (2002). Are planning, working memory, and inhibition associated with individual differences in preschool ADHD symptoms? Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 255–272.PubMedCrossRef Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Dalen, L., Daley, D., & Remington, B. (2002). Are planning, working memory, and inhibition associated with individual differences in preschool ADHD symptoms? Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 255–272.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sprafkin, J., Gadow, K. D., Salisbury, H., Schneider, J., & Loney, J. (2002). Further evidence of reliability and validity of the Child Symptom Inventory-4: parent checklist in clinically referred boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 513–524.PubMed Sprafkin, J., Gadow, K. D., Salisbury, H., Schneider, J., & Loney, J. (2002). Further evidence of reliability and validity of the Child Symptom Inventory-4: parent checklist in clinically referred boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 513–524.PubMed
go back to reference Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically-Based Tests for the Number of Common Factors. Paper presented at the annual Spring meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA. Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically-Based Tests for the Number of Common Factors. Paper presented at the annual Spring meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.
go back to reference Sulik, M. J., Huerta, S., Zerr, A. A., Eisenberg, A., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Di Giunta, L., Pina, A. A., Eggum, N. D., Sallquist, D., Edwards, A., Kupfer, A., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Wilson, S. B., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Taylor, H. B. (2010). The factor structure of effortful control and measurement invariance across ethnicity and sex in a high-risk sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 8–22.CrossRef Sulik, M. J., Huerta, S., Zerr, A. A., Eisenberg, A., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Di Giunta, L., Pina, A. A., Eggum, N. D., Sallquist, D., Edwards, A., Kupfer, A., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Wilson, S. B., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Taylor, H. B. (2010). The factor structure of effortful control and measurement invariance across ethnicity and sex in a high-risk sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 8–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, R. A., Flood, M. F., & Lundquist, L. (1995). Emotional regulation and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (Emotion, cognition, and representation, Vol. 6, pp. 261–299). Rochester: University of Rochester Press. Thompson, R. A., Flood, M. F., & Lundquist, L. (1995). Emotional regulation and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (Emotion, cognition, and representation, Vol. 6, pp. 261–299). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.
go back to reference Valla, J. P. (2000). The Dominic Interactive: Instruction Manual. Montreal, QC: D.I.M.A.T. Inc. Valla, J. P. (2000). The Dominic Interactive: Instruction Manual. Montreal, QC: D.I.M.A.T. Inc.
go back to reference Valla, J. P., Bergeron, L., & Smolla, N. (2000). The Dominic-R: a pictorial interview for 6- to 11-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 85–93.PubMedCrossRef Valla, J. P., Bergeron, L., & Smolla, N. (2000). The Dominic-R: a pictorial interview for 6- to 11-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 85–93.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452.PubMedCrossRef Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Effortful Control in “Hot” and “Cool” Tasks Differentially Predicts Children’s Behavior Problems and Academic Performance
Auteurs
Sanghag Kim
Jamie Koenig Nordling
Jeung Eun Yoon
Lea J. Boldt
Grazyna Kochanska
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology / Uitgave 1/2013
Print ISSN: 2730-7166
Elektronisch ISSN: 2730-7174
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2013

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1/2013 Naar de uitgave