Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2013

01-03-2013 | Original Article

Effects of feature integration in a hands-crossed version of the Social Simon paradigm

Auteurs: Roman Liepelt, Dorit Wenke, Rico Fischer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In previous research, hands-crossed versions of a social variant of the Simon task were used to distinguish between effector-based coding of the Social Simon effect (SSE, analogously to the standard Simon effect) or body-based coding, in which the coding of stimulus location and seating position of the participants functions as a spatial reference frame. In the present study, the analysis of the SSE with respect to previous task requirements (i.e., Simon compatibility in N−1) in a hands-crossed variant of the Social Simon task shows that neither type of coding provides a sole explanation of the pattern of a SSE. Instead, the data pattern seems to be explained more parsimoniously by the assumption of a strengthening of low level feature integration mechanisms in a social setting, taking repetitions and alternations of both agents’ stimulus and response features into account.
Voetnoten
1
In order to better compare our findings with those of the Welsh (2009) study, we ran an additional analysis on the present experimental data with crossed hands including the dataset of a previous study in which another group of participants performed exactly the same task with hands uncrossed (Liepelt et al., 2011). This analysis included the factors Compatibility (Compatible vs. Incompatible), Preceding Compatibility on trial (N−1), and Group (Crossed vs. Uncrossed). The analysis was performed separately for the Individual go/nogo task and the Joint go/nogo task. For the Individual go/nogo task this analysis showed no overall SSE effect, but a sequential modulation across both groups (Crossed, Uncrossed), F(1, 46) = 50.96, MSe = 132.71, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.53. The sequential modulation did not differ between crossed- and uncrossed-effector conditions for the Individual go/nogo tasks, F(1, 46) < 1, partial η2 = 0.01. For the Joint go/nogo tasks, however, we found a significant overall SSE across both groups (Crossed, Uncrossed), F(1, 46) = 7.26, MSe = 156.07, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.14, which was, however, entirely driven by the 9 ms Simon effect in the uncrossed-effector condition (compared to the 0 ms in the hands-crossed condition). This enlargement of the SSE in the uncrossed-effector condition compared to the crossed-effector condition was also reliable, F(1, 46) = 5.27, MSe = 156.07, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.10. Further, we found a sequential modulation across both groups (Crossed, Uncrossed) for the Joint go/nogo tasks, F(1, 46) = 174.44, MSe = 100.66, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.79, which did not differ between crossed- and uncrossed-effector conditions, F(1, 46) < 1, partial η2 = 0.002.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Adamson, L., Bakeman, R., & Dekner, D. (2004). The development of symbol infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75, 1171–1187.PubMedCrossRef Adamson, L., Bakeman, R., & Dekner, D. (2004). The development of symbol infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75, 1171–1187.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.PubMedCrossRef Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T., Barch, D. M., Carter, C., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T., Barch, D. M., Carter, C., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–564.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–564.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402, 179–181.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402, 179–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44, 124–143.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44, 124–143.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2001). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.CrossRef Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2001). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.CrossRef
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How social is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–9.CrossRef Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How social is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, R., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: Conflict-adaptation effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 712–718.PubMedCrossRef Fischer, R., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: Conflict-adaptation effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 712–718.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, R., Plessow, F., Kunde, W., & Kiesel, A. (2010). Trial-to-trial modulations of the Simon effect in conditions of attentional limitations: Evidence from dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1576–1594.PubMedCrossRef Fischer, R., Plessow, F., Kunde, W., & Kiesel, A. (2010). Trial-to-trial modulations of the Simon effect in conditions of attentional limitations: Evidence from dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1576–1594.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 11–21.PubMedCrossRef Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 11–21.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 493–501.CrossRef Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 493–501.CrossRef
go back to reference Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. A. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114, 348–355.PubMedCrossRef Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. A. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114, 348–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations. Psychological Science, 20, 794–798.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations. Psychological Science, 20, 794–798.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010a). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57, 221–227.PubMedCrossRef Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010a). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57, 221–227.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010b). Automatic imitation of physically impossible movements. Social Cognition, 28, 59–74.CrossRef Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010b). Automatic imitation of physically impossible movements. Social Cognition, 28, 59–74.CrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2010). When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions. Cognition, 115, 426–434.PubMedCrossRef Liepelt, R., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2010). When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions. Cognition, 115, 426–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2008a). What is matched in direct matching? Intention attribution modulates motor priming effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 578–591.PubMedCrossRef Liepelt, R., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2008a). What is matched in direct matching? Intention attribution modulates motor priming effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 578–591.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2008b). How do we infer others’ goals from nonstereotypic actions? The outcome of context-sensitive inferential processing in right inferior parietal and posterior temporal cortex. NeuroImage, 43, 784–792.PubMedCrossRef Liepelt, R., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2008b). How do we infer others’ goals from nonstereotypic actions? The outcome of context-sensitive inferential processing in right inferior parietal and posterior temporal cortex. NeuroImage, 43, 784–792.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., Wenke, D., Fischer, R., & Prinz, W. (2011). Trial-to-trial sequential dependencies in a social and non-social Simon task. Psychological Research, 75, 366–375.PubMedCrossRef Liepelt, R., Wenke, D., Fischer, R., & Prinz, W. (2011). Trial-to-trial sequential dependencies in a social and non-social Simon task. Psychological Research, 75, 366–375.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schilbach, L., Wohlschlaeger, A. M., Newen, A., Krämer, N. C., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., et al. (2006). Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction. Neuropsychologia, 44, 718–730.PubMedCrossRef Schilbach, L., Wohlschlaeger, A. M., Newen, A., Krämer, N. C., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., et al. (2006). Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction. Neuropsychologia, 44, 718–730.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint actions: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 70–76.PubMedCrossRef Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint actions: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 70–76.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 353–367.CrossRef Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 353–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21.PubMedCrossRef Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus–response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1234–1246.PubMedCrossRef Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus–response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1234–1246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor, T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: an integrated perspective. Advances in Psychology (Vol. 65, pp. 31–86) Amsterdam: North-Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor, T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: an integrated perspective. Advances in Psychology (Vol. 65, pp. 31–86) Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Simon, J. R., Hinrichs, J. V., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Auditory S–R compatibility: reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 97–102.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., Hinrichs, J. V., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Auditory S–R compatibility: reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 97–102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S–R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S–R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Springer, A., Brandstädter, S., Liepelt, R., Birngruber, T., Giese, M., Mechsner, F., et al. (2011). Motor execution affects action prediction. Brain and Cognition, 76, 26–36.PubMedCrossRef Springer, A., Brandstädter, S., Liepelt, R., Birngruber, T., Giese, M., Mechsner, F., et al. (2011). Motor execution affects action prediction. Brain and Cognition, 76, 26–36.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon Task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363.PubMedCrossRef Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon Task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tsai, C. C., & Brass, M. (2007). Does the human motor system simulate Pinocchio’s actions? Co-acting with a human hand versus a wooden hand in a dyadic interaction. Psychological Science, 18, 1058–1062.PubMedCrossRef Tsai, C. C., & Brass, M. (2007). Does the human motor system simulate Pinocchio’s actions? Co-acting with a human hand versus a wooden hand in a dyadic interaction. Psychological Science, 18, 1058–1062.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2008). Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2015–2024.PubMedCrossRef Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2008). Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2015–2024.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Jing, J. T., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). The common coding framework in self-other interaction: Evidence from joint action task. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 353–362.CrossRef Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Jing, J. T., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). The common coding framework in self-other interaction: Evidence from joint action task. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 353–362.CrossRef
go back to reference Vlainic, E., Liepelt, R., Colzato, L. S., Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (2010). The virtual co-actor: the Social Simon effect does not rely on online feedback from the other. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–6.CrossRef Vlainic, E., Liepelt, R., Colzato, L. S., Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (2010). The virtual co-actor: the Social Simon effect does not rely on online feedback from the other. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–6.CrossRef
go back to reference Welsh, T. N. (2009). When 1 + 1 = 1: the unification of independent actors revealed through joint Simon effects persist in crossed and uncrossed effector conditions. Human Movement Science, 28, 726–737.PubMedCrossRef Welsh, T. N. (2009). When 1 + 1 = 1: the unification of independent actors revealed through joint Simon effects persist in crossed and uncrossed effector conditions. Human Movement Science, 28, 726–737.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wenke, D., Holländer, A., Atmaca, S., Liepelt, R., Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2011). What is shared in joint action? The contents of co-representation. The Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2, 147–172.CrossRef Wenke, D., Holländer, A., Atmaca, S., Liepelt, R., Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2011). What is shared in joint action? The contents of co-representation. The Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2, 147–172.CrossRef
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 71, 401–410.PubMedCrossRef Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 71, 401–410.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Winkel, J., Wijnen, J. G., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Groen, I., Derrfuss, J., Danielmeier, C., et al. (2009). Your conflict matters to me! Behavioral and neural manifestations of control adjustments after self-experienced and observed decision-conflict. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 1–8.CrossRef Winkel, J., Wijnen, J. G., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Groen, I., Derrfuss, J., Danielmeier, C., et al. (2009). Your conflict matters to me! Behavioral and neural manifestations of control adjustments after self-experienced and observed decision-conflict. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 1–8.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Effects of feature integration in a hands-crossed version of the Social Simon paradigm
Auteurs
Roman Liepelt
Dorit Wenke
Rico Fischer
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2013
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0425-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2013

Psychological Research 2/2013 Naar de uitgave