Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2005

01-03-2005 | Original Article

Effector dependent sequence learning in the serial RT task

Auteurs: Willem B. Verwey, Benjamin A. Clegg

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2005

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

At least five earlier studies could not find effector-dependent learning in the keying version of the serial reaction time (RT) task. Experiment 1 examined whether effector-dependent learning occurs when participants practice the serial RT task with three fingers of one hand for about 1,300 sequence repetitions instead of the more common 50–100 repetitions. The results confirm that, following extended practice, sequence learning produces an effector-dependent component. Specifically, an unpracticed hand executed a practiced sequence slower than a practiced hand. However, Experiment 2 showed that effector-dependent sequence learning develops only when fingers of one hand are used, suggesting that effector-dependent sequence learning involves adjustment to the mechanical interactions between the fingers of one hand. In addition, when sequences had been practiced with one hand, mirror versions of the practiced sequences in both experiments showed moderate transfer. But when practiced with two hands no transfer to a mirrored version of the sequence was observed. This suggests that only practice with one hand produces a representation that facilitates the execution of mirror sequences. Generally, the same results were found in more or less aware participants, congruent with the idea that the effector-dependent representation and the representation allowing transfer to mirror sequences are implicit.
Voetnoten
1
As the frequencies of the individual keys were unequal, part of the learning effect can be explained by frequency learning. However, frequency learning still implies an effector-independent effect and, hence, cannot be responsible for effector-dependent sequence learning.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000) Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149–162. Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000) Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149–162.
go back to reference Berkinblit, M. B., & Feldman, A. G. (1988). Some problems of motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior, 20, 369–373.PubMed Berkinblit, M. B., & Feldman, A. G. (1988). Some problems of motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior, 20, 369–373.PubMed
go back to reference Buchner, A., Steffens, M. C., Erdfelder, E., & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinomial model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 631–663. Buchner, A., Steffens, M. C., Erdfelder, E., & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinomial model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 631–663.
go back to reference Clohessy, A. B., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Development of the functional visual field. Acta Psychologica, 106, 51–68.CrossRefPubMed Clohessy, A. B., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Development of the functional visual field. Acta Psychologica, 106, 51–68.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30. Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30.
go back to reference Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Donchin, O., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Shadmehr, R. (2003). Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 168–176.PubMed Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Donchin, O., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Shadmehr, R. (2003). Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 168–176.PubMed
go back to reference Eimer, M., Goschke, T., Schlaghecken, F., & Stürmer, B. (1996). Explicit and implicit learning of event sequences: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 970–987. Eimer, M., Goschke, T., Schlaghecken, F., & Stürmer, B. (1996). Explicit and implicit learning of event sequences: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 970–987.
go back to reference Gentner, D. R. (1987). Timing of skilled motor performance: Test of the proportional duration model. Psychological Review, 94, 255–276.CrossRef Gentner, D. R. (1987). Timing of skilled motor performance: Test of the proportional duration model. Psychological Review, 94, 255–276.CrossRef
go back to reference Gentner, D. R., Larochelle, S., & Grudin, J. T. (1988). Lexical, sublexical, and peripheral effects in skilled typewriting. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 524–548. Gentner, D. R., Larochelle, S., & Grudin, J. T. (1988). Lexical, sublexical, and peripheral effects in skilled typewriting. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 524–548.
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (1995). Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 497–510. Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (1995). Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 497–510.
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (1998). Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 9420–9428.PubMed Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (1998). Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 9420–9428.PubMed
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand. A PET functional imaging study. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 369–378.CrossRef Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand. A PET functional imaging study. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 369–378.CrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1996). Coordination. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. 2 (pp. 121–180). London: Academic. Heuer, H. (1996). Coordination. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. 2 (pp. 121–180). London: Academic.
go back to reference Heyes, C. M., & Foster, C. L. (2002). Motor learning by observation. Evidence from a serial reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 593–607. Heyes, C. M., & Foster, C. L. (2002). Motor learning by observation. Evidence from a serial reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 593–607.
go back to reference Hicks, R. E. (1974). Asymmetry of bilateral transfer. American Journal of Psychology, 87, 667–674. Hicks, R. E. (1974). Asymmetry of bilateral transfer. American Journal of Psychology, 87, 667–674.
go back to reference Hikosaka, O., Nakahara, H., Rand, M. K., Sakai, K., Lu, X., Nakamura, K., Miyachi, S., & Doya, K. (1999). Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 465–471. Hikosaka, O., Nakahara, H., Rand, M. K., Sakai, K., Lu, X., Nakamura, K., Miyachi, S., & Doya, K. (1999). Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 465–471.
go back to reference Hikosaka, O., Sakai, K., Nakahara, H., Lu, X., Miyachi, S., Nakamura, K., & Rand, M. K. (2000). Neural mechanisms for learning of sequential procedures. In M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 553–572). Cambridge, MA: MIT. Hikosaka, O., Sakai, K., Nakahara, H., Lu, X., Miyachi, S., Nakamura, K., & Rand, M. K. (2000). Neural mechanisms for learning of sequential procedures. In M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 553–572). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
go back to reference Hlûstik, P., Solodkin, A., Gullapalli, R. P., Noll, D. C., & Small, S. L. (2002). Functional lateralization of the human premotor cortex during sequential movements. Brain & Cognition, 49, 54–62. Hlûstik, P., Solodkin, A., Gullapalli, R. P., Noll, D. C., & Small, S. L. (2002). Functional lateralization of the human premotor cortex during sequential movements. Brain & Cognition, 49, 54–62.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in SRT tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482. Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in SRT tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.
go back to reference Howard, J. H., Mutter, S. A., & Howard, D. V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1029–1039. Howard, J. H., Mutter, S. A., & Howard, D. V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1029–1039.
go back to reference Jordan, M. I. (1995). The organization of action sequences: Evidence from a relearning task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 179–192.PubMed Jordan, M. I. (1995). The organization of action sequences: Evidence from a relearning task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 179–192.PubMed
go back to reference Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: Fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 861–868.CrossRef Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: Fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 861–868.CrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30. Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Ivry, R. B., Hazeltine, E., Mayr, U., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representations. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.CrossRefPubMed Keele, S. W., Ivry, R. B., Hazeltine, E., Mayr, U., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representations. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kent, R. D., & Minifie, F. D. (1977). Coarticulation in recent speech production models. Journal of Phonetics, 5, 115–133. Kent, R. D., & Minifie, F. D. (1977). Coarticulation in recent speech production models. Journal of Phonetics, 5, 115–133.
go back to reference Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863–882. Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863–882.
go back to reference Kornbrot, D. E. (1989). Organization of keying skills: The effect of motor complexity and number of units. Acta Psychologica, 70, 19–41.CrossRef Kornbrot, D. E. (1989). Organization of keying skills: The effect of motor complexity and number of units. Acta Psychologica, 70, 19–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Lahy, J. M. (1924). Motion study in typewriting. In Studies and reports: Series J (Educational), 3. Geneva: International Labour Office. Lahy, J. M. (1924). Motion study in typewriting. In Studies and reports: Series J (Educational), 3. Geneva: International Labour Office.
go back to reference Lindemann, P. G., & Wright, C. E. (1998). Skill acquisition and plans for actions: Learning to write with your other hand. In D. Scarborough & S. Sternberg (Eds.), Methods, models, and conceptual issues. An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. 4 (pp. 523–584). Cambridge, MA: MIT. Lindemann, P. G., & Wright, C. E. (1998). Skill acquisition and plans for actions: Learning to write with your other hand. In D. Scarborough & S. Sternberg (Eds.), Methods, models, and conceptual issues. An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. 4 (pp. 523–584). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
go back to reference MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89, 483–506.CrossRef MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89, 483–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364. Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364.
go back to reference McDowell, M. J., & Wolff, P. H. (1997). A functional analysis of human mirror movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 85–96. McDowell, M. J., & Wolff, P. H. (1997). A functional analysis of human mirror movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 85–96.
go back to reference Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98–112. Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98–112.
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32. Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.
go back to reference Park, J.-H., & Shea, C. H. (2003). Effect of practice on effector independence. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 33–40.PubMed Park, J.-H., & Shea, C. H. (2003). Effect of practice on effector independence. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 33–40.PubMed
go back to reference Parker-Taillon, D., & Kerr, R. (1989). Manual asymmetries within the performance of a complex motor task. Human Movement Science, 8, 33–44.CrossRef Parker-Taillon, D., & Kerr, R. (1989). Manual asymmetries within the performance of a complex motor task. Human Movement Science, 8, 33–44.CrossRef
go back to reference Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., & Hallett, M. (1994). Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science, 263, 1286–1289. Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., & Hallett, M. (1994). Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science, 263, 1286–1289.
go back to reference Rand, M. K., Hirosaka, O., Miyachi, S., Lu, X., Nakamura, K., Kitaguchi, K., & Shimo, Y. (2000). Characteristics of sequential movements during early learning in monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 131, 293–304.CrossRef Rand, M. K., Hirosaka, O., Miyachi, S., Lu, X., Nakamura, K., Kitaguchi, K., & Shimo, Y. (2000). Characteristics of sequential movements during early learning in monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 131, 293–304.CrossRef
go back to reference Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 584–594. Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 584–594.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. J., & Vaughan, J. (1999). Remembered positions: Stored locations or stored postures? Experimental Brain Research, 124, 503–512.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. J., & Vaughan, J. (1999). Remembered positions: Stored locations or stored postures? Experimental Brain Research, 124, 503–512.CrossRef
go back to reference Rüsseler, J., & Rösler, F. (2000). Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. Acta Psychologica, 104, 45–67.CrossRefPubMed Rüsseler, J., & Rösler, F. (2000). Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. Acta Psychologica, 104, 45–67.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmuckler, M. A., & Bosman, E. L. (1997). Interkey timing in piano performance and typing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 99–111.PubMed Schmuckler, M. A., & Bosman, E. L. (1997). Interkey timing in piano performance and typing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 99–111.PubMed
go back to reference Schneider, W., Zuccolotto, A., & Tirone, S. T. (1993). Time-stamping computer events to report 1-msec accuracy of events in the Micro Experimental Laboratory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25, 276–280. Schneider, W., Zuccolotto, A., & Tirone, S. T. (1993). Time-stamping computer events to report 1-msec accuracy of events in the Micro Experimental Laboratory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25, 276–280.
go back to reference Shaffer, L. H. (1975). Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V (pp. 157–167). New York: Academic. Shaffer, L. H. (1975). Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V (pp. 157–167). New York: Academic.
go back to reference Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069. Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069.
go back to reference Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic. Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic.
go back to reference Verwey, W. B., & Wright, D. (2004). Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task. Psychological Research, 68, 64–70.CrossRefPubMed Verwey, W. B., & Wright, D. (2004). Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task. Psychological Research, 68, 64–70.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wachs, K., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., & Hallett, M. (1994). Intermanual transfer of implicit knowledge of sequential finger movements. Neurology, 44, Supplement 2, 805S. Wachs, K., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., & Hallett, M. (1994). Intermanual transfer of implicit knowledge of sequential finger movements. Neurology, 44, Supplement 2, 805S.
go back to reference Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.CrossRefPubMed Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory and Cognition, 27, 561–572. Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory and Cognition, 27, 561–572.
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1047–1060. Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1047–1060.
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366–375. Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366–375.
go back to reference Wolff, P. H., Gunnoe, C. E., & Cohen, C. (1983). Associated movements as a measure of developmental age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 417–429.PubMed Wolff, P. H., Gunnoe, C. E., & Cohen, C. (1983). Associated movements as a measure of developmental age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 417–429.PubMed
go back to reference Zirngibl, C., & Koch, I. (2002). The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 153–162.CrossRefPubMed Zirngibl, C., & Koch, I. (2002). The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 153–162.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Effector dependent sequence learning in the serial RT task
Auteurs
Willem B. Verwey
Benjamin A. Clegg
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2005
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2005
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0181-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2005

Psychological Research 4/2005 Naar de uitgave