Skip to main content
Research Article

The Mere Exposure Instruction Effect

Mere Exposure Instructions Influence Liking

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000376

Abstract. The mere exposure effect refers to the well-established finding that people evaluate a stimulus more positively after repeated exposure to that stimulus. We investigated whether a change in stimulus evaluation can occur also when participants are not repeatedly exposed to a stimulus, but are merely instructed that one stimulus will occur frequently and another stimulus will occur infrequently. We report seven experiments showing that (1) mere exposure instructions influence implicit stimulus evaluations as measured with an Implicit Association Test (IAT), personalized Implicit Association Test (pIAT), or Affect Misattribution Procedure (AMP), but not with an Evaluative Priming Task (EPT), (2) mere exposure instructions influence explicit evaluations, and (3) the instruction effect depends on participants’ memory of which stimulus will be presented more frequently. We discuss how these findings inform us about the boundary conditions of mere exposure instruction effects, as well as the mental processes that underlie mere exposure and mere exposure instruction effects.

References

  • Allport, G. (1935). Attitudes. In C. MurchisonEd., A handbook of social psychology (pp. 789–843). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Alter, A., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N. & Eyre, R. (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytical thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569–576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4, R package v. 1.0-6. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–289. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bornstein, R. F. & D’Agostino, P. R. (1992). Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 545–552. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.54 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Brooks, J. O. & Watkins, M. J. (1989). Recognition memory and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 968–976. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.968 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2003). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. In J. MuschK. C. KlauerEds., The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 219–244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2006). Using the Implicit Association Test does not rule out an impact of conscious propositional knowledge on evaluative conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 37, 176–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2005.12.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2007). A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative conditioning. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600006491 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2009a). How do people evaluate objects? A brief review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 36–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00162.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2009b). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.3758/LB.37.1.1 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2014). A propositional model of implicit evaluation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 342–353. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12111 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J., Beckers, T. & Moors, A. (2007). Novel attitudes can be faked on the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 972–978. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. & Hughes, S. (2016). Evaluative conditioning as a symbolic phenomenon: On the relation between evaluative conditioning, evaluative conditioning via instructions, and persuasion. Social Cognition, 34, 480–494. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2016.34.5.480 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A. & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014211 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • de Zilva, D., Vu, L., Newell, B. R. & Pearson, J. (2013). Exposure is not enough: Suppressing stimuli from awareness can abolish the mere exposure effect. PLoS One, 8, e77726. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077726 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Ebert, I. D., Steffens, M. C., von Stülpnagel, R. & Jelenec, P. (2009). How to like yourself better, or chocolate less: Changing implicit attitudes with one IAT task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1098–1104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fang, X., Singh, S. & Ahluwalia, R. (2007). An examination of different explanations for the mere exposure effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1086/513050 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C. & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fechner, G. T. (1876). Vorschule der Aesthetik [Introduction to esthetics]. Leipzig, Germany: Breitkopf and Härtel. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Field, A. P. (2006). Is conditioning a useful framework for understanding the development and treatment of phobias? Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 857–875. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2005.05.010 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K. & Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control on the Implicit Association Tests. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 307–316. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gast, A. & De Houwer, J. (2012). Evaluative conditioning without directly experienced pairings of the conditioned and the unconditioned stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1657–1674. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.665061 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. ReisC. M. JuddEds., Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 283–310). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. & Ye, Y. (2014). What drives priming effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213502548 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Glenberg, A. M. & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196313 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E. & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A. & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Harmon-Jones, E. & Allen, J. J. B. (2001). The role of affect in the mere exposure effect: Evidence from psycho-physiological and individual differences approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 889–898. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201277011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F. & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390–421. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018916 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, S., Van Dessel, P. & Smith, C. T. (2017). A comparative analysis of different evaluative learning procedures. Manuscript in preparation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Millisecond Software LLC. (2011). Inquisit 3.0. [Computer Software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software LLC. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.110.3.306 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kawakami, K., Phills, C. E., Steele, J. R. & Dovidio, J. F. (2007). (Close) distance makes the heart grow fonder: Improving implicit racial evaluations and interracial interactions through approach behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 957–971. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.957 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kinoshita, S. & Peek-O’Leary, M. (2006). Two bases of the compatibility effect in the Implicit Association Test (IAT). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 2102–2120. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210500451141 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krieglmeyer, R. & Sherman, J. W. (2012). Disentangling stereotype activation and stereotype application in the Stereotype Misperception Task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 205–224. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W. & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberative judgements are based on common principles. Psychological Review, 118, 97–109. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kunst-Wilson, W. & Zajonc, R. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7352271 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kurdi, B. & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Repeated evaluative pairings and evaluative statements: How effectively do they shift implicit attitudes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146, 194–213. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000239 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Maslow, A. H. (1937). The influence of familiarization on preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 162–180. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0053692 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meissner, F. & Rothermund, K. (2013). Estimating the contributions of associations and recoding in the Implicit Association Test: The ReAL model for the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 45–69. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030734 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T. & Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychological Science, 11, 462–466. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00289 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Newell, B. R. & Shanks, D. R. (2007). Recognising what you like: Examining the relation between the mere-exposure effect and recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 103–118. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440500487454 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565–584. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, M. A. & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extra-personal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653–667. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.653 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O. & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. & Gawronski, B. (2010). A history of implicit social cognition: Where is it coming from? Where is it now? Where is it going?. In B. GawronskiB. K. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 1–15). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Raes, A. K., De Houwer, J., De Schryver, M., Brass, M. & Kalisch, R. (2014). Do CS-US pairings actually matter? A within-subject comparison of instructed fear conditioning with and without actual CS-US pairings. PLoS One, 9, e84888. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084888 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Reber, R., Schwarz, N. & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–382. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Reber, R., Winkielman, P. & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9, 45–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Gast, A. & Wentura, D. (2009). Minimizing the influence of recoding in the Implicit Association Test: The Recoding-Free Implicit Association Test (IAT-RF). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 84–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701822975 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K. & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 139–165. https://doi.org/10.1037/00963445.133.2.139 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D. & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Sawyer, A. G. (1975). Detecting demand characteristics in laboratory experiments in consumer research: The case of repetition-affect research. In M. J. SchlingerEd., Advances in consumer research (Vol. 2, pp. 713–724). Ann Abor, MI: Association for Consumer Research. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C. T., De Houwer, J. & Nosek, B. (2013). Consider the source: persuasion of implicit evaluations is moderated by source credibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 193–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212472374 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J. & Hermans, D. (2009). Modulation of semantic priming by feature-specific attention allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 37–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D. & Eelen, P. (2007). Affective priming of non-affective semantic categorization responses. Experimental Psychology, 54, 44–53. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.44 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Stafford, T. & Grimes, A. (2012). Memory enhances the mere exposure effect. Psychology & Marketing, 29(12), 995–1003. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20581 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Storbeck, J. & Robinson, M. D. (2004). When preferences need inferences: A direct comparison of the automaticity of cognitive versus affective priming. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 81–93. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Teige-Mocigemba, S. & Klauer, K. C. (2013). On the controllability of evaluative-priming effects: Some limits that are none. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 632–657. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., Penzl, B., Becker, M., Henn, L. & Klauer, K. C. (2016). Controlling the “uncontrollable”: Faking effects on the Affect Misattribution Procedure. Cognition & Emotion, 30, 1470–1484. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Topolinski, S. (2012). The sensorimotor contributions to implicit memory, familiarity, and recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 260–281. https://doi.org/10.1037/a002565 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J. & Gast, A. (2016). Approach-Avoidance Training effects are Moderated by Awareness of Stimulus-Action Contingencies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 81–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215615335 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J., Gast, A. & Smith, C. T. (2015). Instruction-based approach-avoidance effects: Changing stimulus evaluation via mere instruction to approach or avoid stimuli. Experimental Psychology, 62, 161–169. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000282 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J., Gast, A., Smith, C. T. & De Schryver, M. (2016). Instructing implicit processes: When instructions to approach or avoid influence implicit but not explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.11.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J., Roets, A. & Gast, A. (2016). Failures to change stimulus evaluations by means of subliminal approach and avoidance training. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, e1–e15. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000039 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J. & Smith, C. T. (2017). Relational information moderates approach-avoidance instruction effects on implicit evaluation. Acta Psychologica, Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.03.016 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., Gawronski, B., Smith, C. T. & De Houwer, J. (2017). Mechanisms underlying approach-avoidance instruction effects on implicit evaluation: Results of a preregistered adversarial collaboration. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 23–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.10.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dessel, P., Mertens, G., Smith, C. T. & De Houwer, J. (2017). Mere exposure effects on implicit evaluation: The moderating role of evaluation task, number of stimulus presentations and memory of stimulus presentations. Manuscript in preparation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, M.-Y. & Chang, H.-C. (2004). The mere exposure effect and recognition memory. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 1055–1078. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930341000374 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wentura, D. & Degner, J. (2010). A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In B. GawronskiB. K. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 95–116). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. WittenbrinkN. SchwarzEds., Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 17–58). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Woud, M. L., Maas, J., Becker, E. S. & Rinck, M. (2013). Make the manikin move: Symbolic approach-avoidance responses affect implicit and explicit face evaluations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 738–744. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.817413 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monographs, 9, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 224–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.0015428 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zanon, R., De Houwer, J., Gast, A. & Smith, C. T. (2014). When does relational information influence evaluative conditioning? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 2105–2122. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.907324 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhou, H. & Fishbach, A. (2016). The pitfall of experimenting on the web: How unattended selective attrition leads to surprising (yet false) research conclusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 493–504. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000056 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar