Skip to main content
Multistudy Report

Assessing Positive Orientation With the Implicit Association Test

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000362

Abstract. Positive orientation (PO) is a basic predisposition that consists in a positive outlook toward oneself, one’s life, and one’s future, which is associated to many desirable outcomes connected to health and to the general quality of life. We performed a lexical study for identifying a set of markers of PO, developed an Implicit Association Test (the PO-IAT), and investigated its psychometric properties. The PO-IAT proved to be a reliable measure with a clear pattern of convergent validity, both with respect to self-report scales connected to PO and with respect to an indirect measure of self-esteem. A secondary aim of our studies was to validate a new brief adjective scale to assess PO, the POAS. Our results show that both the PO-IAT and the self-reported PO predict the frequency of depressive symptoms and of self-perceived intelligence.

References

  • Alessandri, G., Caprara, G. V. & Tisak, J. (2012). The unique contribution of positive orientation to optimal functioning: Further explorations. European Psychologist, 17, 44–54. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000070 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Alessandri, G., Vecchione, M., Tisak, J., Deiana, G., Caria, S. & Caprara, G. V. (2012). The utility of positive orientation in predicting job performance and organisational citizenship behaviors. Applied Psychology, 61, 669–698. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2012.00511.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Asendorpf, J. B., Conner, M., De Fruyt, F., De Houwer, J., Denissen, J. J. A., Fiedler, K., … Wicherts, J. M. (2013). Recommendations for increasing replicability in psychology. European Journal of Personality, 27, 108–119. doi: 10.1002/per.1919 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C. & Egloff, B. (2005). Measuring task-switching ability in the Implicit Association Test. Experimental Psychology, 52, 167–179. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.167 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C. & Egloff, B. (2009). Predicting actual behavior from the explicit and implicit self-concept of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 533–548. doi: 10.1037/a0016229 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bluemke, M. & Friese, M. (2006). Do features of stimuli influence IAT effects? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 163–176. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caprara, G. V., Alessandri, G., Colaiaco, F. & Zuffianò, A. (2013). Dispositional bases of self-serving positive evaluations. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 864–867. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.465 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caprara, G. V., Alessandri, G., Eisenberg, N., Kupfer, A., Steca, P., Caprara, M. G., … Abela, J. (2012). The positivity scale. Psychological Assessment, 24, 701–712. doi: 10.1037/a0026681 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costantini, G., Richetin, J., Borsboom, D., Fried, E. I., Rhemtulla, M. & Perugini, M. (2015). Development of indirect measures of conscientiousness: Combining a facets approach and network analysis. European Journal of Personality, 29, 548–567. doi: 10.1002/per.2014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. & Moors, A. (2010). Implicit measures: Similarities and differences. In B. GawronskiB. K. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 176–193). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J. & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fava, G. A. (1983). Assessing depressive symptoms across cultures: Italian validation of the CES-D self-rating scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 249–251. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198303)39:2<249::AID-JCLP2270390218>3.0.CO;2-Y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furnham, A. & Buchanan, T. (2005). Personality, gender and self-perceived intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 543–555. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E. & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A. & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L. & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41. doi: 10.1037/a0015575 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heikamp, T., Alessandri, G., Laguna, M., Petrovic, V., Caprara, M. G. & Trommsdorff, G. (2014). Cross-cultural validation of the positivity-scale in five European countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 71, 140–145. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lauriola, M. & Iani, L. (2015). Does positivity mediate the relation of extraversion and neuroticism with subjective happiness? PLoS One, 10, e0121991. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121991 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lorenzo-Seva, U. & ten Berge, J. M. F. (2006). Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology, 2, 57–64. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.2.2.57 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K., Burkley, M. A. & Stokes, M. B. (2008). Why do implicit and explicit attitude tests diverge? The role of structural fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 16–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.16 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M., Costantini, G., Richetin, J. & Zogmaister, C. (2015). Implicit association tests, then and now. In F. J. R. val de VijverT. M. OrtnerEds., Behavior based assessment: Going beyond self report in the personality, affective, motivation, and social domains (pp. 15–28). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe Publishing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M., Richetin, J. & Zogmaister, C. (2010). Prediction of Behavior. In B. GawronskiB. K. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition (pp. 255–278). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S. & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D. & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Buuren, S. & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45, 1–67. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Velicer, W. F. (1976). Determining the number of components from the matrix of partial correlations. Psychometrika, 41, 321–327. doi: 10.1007/BF02293557 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A. & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zeigler-Hill, V. (2006). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit self-esteem: implications for narcissism and self-esteem instability. Journal of Personality, 74, 119–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00371.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ziegler, M. (2014). Stop and state your intentions!. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 30, 239–242. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000228 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar