Skip to main content
Article

Neuro-Behavioral Correlates of Post-Deviance Distraction in Middle-Aged and Old Adults

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000124

The presentation of a task-irrelevant deviant (novel) stimulus among otherwise repeated standard stimuli usually reduces performance not only for the deviant stimulus, but also for the standard following that deviant. Here, the so-called post-deviance distraction was investigated in 58 middle-aged and 52 old adults, using an auditory duration discrimination task and event-related potential (ERP) measures. After a deviant stimulus, the participants showed a decrease in performance in the subsequent standard stimulus. This effect was more pronounced in the old, than middle-aged, group. Relative to the standard stimuli preceding the deviant, post-deviant standards triggered a chain of mismatch negativity (MMN), P3a, and reorienting negativity (RON). While MMN and P3a did not differ in old and middle-aged adults, older participants showed a delayed RON. Assuming the RON to reflect processes of general task or feature reconfiguration and updating, these results suggest a delay in orienting-reorienting mechanisms as possible source of increased post-deviance distraction in elderly.

References

  • Alain, C. , Woods, D. L. (1999). Age-related changes in processing auditory stimuli during visual attention: Evidence for deficits in inhibitory control and sensory memory. Psychology and Aging, 14, 507–519. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderer, P. , Semlitsch, H. V. , Saletu, B. (1996). Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: Effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 458–472. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 280–289. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barrett, G. , Neshige, R. , Shibasaki, H. (1987). Human auditory and somatosensory event-related potentials: Effects of response condition and age. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 66, 409–419. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berti, S. (2008). Cognitive control after distraction: Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) dissociate between different processes of attentional allocation. Psychophysiology, 45, 608–620. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berti, S. , Grunwald, M. , Schröger, E. (2013). Age dependent changes of distractibility and reorienting of attention revised: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1491, 156–166. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cooper, R. J. , Todd, J. , McGill, K. , Michie, P. T. (2006). Auditory sensory memory and the aging brain: A mismatch negativity study. Neurobiology of Aging, 27, 752–762. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Czigler, I. , Csibra, G. , Csontos, A. (1992). Age and inter-stimulus interval effects on event-related potentials to frequent and infrequent auditory stimuli. Biological Psychology, 33, 195–206. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darwin, C. J. (2008). Listening to speech in the presence of other sounds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 363, 1011–1021. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dennis, N. A. , Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In F. I. M. Craik, T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The Handbook of Aging and Cognition (pp. 1–54). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, J. (2006). EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: Brain mechanisms of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 2–27. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Escera, C. , Alho, K. , Schröger, E. , Winkler, I. (2000). Involuntary attention and distractibility as evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 5, 151–166. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hoormann, J. , Christ, S. , Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51, 87–107. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Folstein, M. F. , Folstein, S. E. , McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Folstein, J. R. , Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152–170. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, D. , Cycowicz, Y. M. , Gaeta, H. (2001). The novelty P3: An event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain’s evaluation of novelty. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 355–373. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gaeta, H. , Friedman, D. , Ritter, W. , Cheng, J. (2001). An event-related potential evaluation of involuntary attentional shifts in young and older adults. Psychology of Aging, 16, 55–68. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gajewski, P. D. , Stoerig, P. , Falkenstein, M. (2008). ERP-correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Research, 1189, 127–134. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gajewski, P. D. , Falkenstein, M. (2012). Training-induced improvement of response selection and error detection in aging assessed by task switching: Effects of cognitive, physical, and relaxation training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 130. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Getzmann, S. (2012). Handicapped due to age? Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of speech perception of dichotically presented narratives in young and middle-aged listeners. Journal of Psychophysiology, 26, 132–144. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Getzmann, S. , Falkenstein, M. (2011). Understanding of spoken language under challenging listening conditions in younger and older listeners: A combined behavioural and electrophysiological study. Brain Research, 1415, 8–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Getzmann, S. , Gajewski, P. D. , Falkenstein, M. (2013). Does age increase auditory distraction? Electrophysiological correlates of high and low performance in seniors. Neurobiology and Aging, 34, 1952–1962. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Getzmann, S. , Falkenstein, M. , Gajewski, P. D. (2013). Long-term cardiovascular fitness is associated with auditory attentional control in old adults: Neuro-behavioral evidence. PLoS One, 8, e74539. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Getzmann, S. , Gajewski, P. D. , Hengstler, J. G. , Falkenstein, M. , Beste, C. (2013). BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and goal-directed behaviour in healthy elderly-evidence from auditory distraction. NeuroImage, 64, 290–298. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gmehlin, D. , Kreisel, S. H. , Bachmann, S. , Weisbrod, M. , Thomas, C. (2011). Age effects on preattentive and early attentive auditory processing of redundant stimuli: Is sensory gating affected by physiological aging? Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 66, 1043–1053. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gratton, G. , Coles, M. G. H. , Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoelig, C. , Berti, S. (2010). To switch or not to switch: Brain potential indices of attentional control after task-relevant and task-irrelevant changes of stimulus features. Brain Research, 1345, 164–174. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horváth, J. , Czigler, I. , Birkás, E. , Winkler, I. , Gervai, J. (2009). Age-related differences in distraction and reorientation in an auditory task. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 1157–1172. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kähkönen, S. , Ahveninen, J. , Pekkonen, E. , Kaakkola, S. , Huttunen, R. J. , Jääskeläinen, I. P. (2002). Dopamine modulates involuntary shifting and reorienting: An electromagnetic study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 1894–1902. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Knight, R. T. , Scabini, D. (1998). Anatomic bases of event-related potentials and their relationship to novelty detection in humans. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 15, 3–13. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kramer, A. F. , Madden, D. J. (2008). Attention. In F. I. M. Craik, T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The Handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 189–249). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ljungberg, J. K. , Parmentier, F. B. R. (2012). Cross-modal distraction by deviance. Functional similarities between the auditory and tactile modalities. Experimental Psychology, 59, 355–363. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Mager, R. , Falkenstein, M. , Störmer, R. , Brand, S. , Müller-Spahn, F. , Bullinger, A. H. (2005). Auditory distraction in young and middle-aged adults: A behavioral and event related potential study. Journal of Neural Transmission, 112, 1165–1176. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Munka, L. , Berti, S. (2006). Examining task-dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event-related brain potential. Neuroscience Letters, 396, 177–181. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. (1990). The role of attention in auditory information-processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function. Behavioural Brain Science, 13, 201–288. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. , Picton, T. W. (1987). The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology, 24, 375–425. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. , Gaillard, A. W. K. , Mantysalo, S. (1978). Early selective attention effect on evoked-potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologia, 42, 313–329. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. , Alho, K. (1995). Mismatch negativity – a unique measure of sensory processing in audition. International Journal of Neuroscience, 80, 317–337. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Novak, G. , Ritter, W. , Vaughan, H. G. Jr. (1992). Mismatch detection and the latency of temporal judgements. Psychophysiology, 29, 398–411. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parmentier, F. B. R. , Andrés, P. (2010). The involuntary capture of attention by sound: Novelty and post-novelty distraction in young and older adults. Experimental Psychology, 57, 68–76. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Parmentier, F. B. R. , Elsley, J. V. , Andrés, P. , Barceló, F. (2011). Why are auditory novels distracting? Contrasting the roles of novelty, violation of expectation and stimulus change. Cognition, 119, 374–380. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pekkonen, E. , Jousmaki, V. , Partanen, J. , Karhu, J. (1993). Mismatch negativity area and age-related auditory memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 87, 321–325. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2008). Use of supportive context by younger and older adult listeners: Balancing bottom-up and top-down information processing. International Journal of Audiology, 47, S72–S82. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128–2148. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Polich, J. , Criado, J. R. (2006). Neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of the P3a and P3b. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 172–185. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Potts, G. F. (2004). An ERP index of task relevance evaluation of visual stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 56, 5–13. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rimmele, J. , Sussman, E. , Keitel, C. , Jacobsen, T. , Schröger, E. (2012). Electrophysiological evidence for age effects on sensory memory processing of tonal patterns. Psychology and Aging, 27, 384–398. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roeber, U. , Berti, S. , Widmann, A. , Schröger, E. (2005). Response repetition vs. response change modulates behavioral and electrophysiological effects of distraction. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 451–456. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roeber, U. , Widmann, A. , Schröger, E. (2003). Auditory distraction by duration and location deviants: A behavioral and event-related potential study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 347–357. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruzzoli, M. , Pirulli, C. , Brignani, D. , Maioli, C. , Miniussi, C. (2012). Sensory memory during physiological aging indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN). Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 625.e21–625.e30. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sams, M. , Alho, K. , Näätänen, R. (1984). Short-term habituation and dishabituation of the mismatch negativity of the ERP. Psychophysiology, 21, 434–441. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, B. A. (2011). How age affects auditory-cognitive interactions in speech comprehension. Audiology Research, 1, e10. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. (1996). A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditory stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 527–539. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Wolff, C. (1998). Attentional orienting and reorienting is indicated by human event-related brain potentials. Neuroreport, 9, 3355–3358. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Giard, M. H. , Wolff, C. (2000). Auditory distraction: Event-related potential and behavioural indices. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 1450–1460. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Bendixen, A. , Trujillo-Barreto, N. J. , Roeber, U. (2007). Processing of abstract rule violations in audition. PLoS One, 2, e1131. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sussman, E. , Winkler, I. , Schröger, E. (2003). Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 630–637. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wascher, E. , Falkenstein, M. , Wild-Wall, N. (2011). Age related strategic differences in processing irrelevant information. Neuroscience Letters, 487, 66–69. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wascher, E. , Schneider, D. , Hoffmann, S. , Beste, C. , Sänger, J. (2012). When compensation fails: Attentional deficits in healthy ageing caused by visual distraction. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3185–3192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wild-Wall, N. , Falkenstein, M. , Gajewski, P. D. (2012). Neural correlates of changes in a visual search task due to cognitive training. Neural Plasticity. ID529057. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Winkler, I. (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 147–163. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar