Skip to main content
Originalarbeit

Emotionsbewusstsein und depressive Symptome im frühen Jugendalter

Zur Bedeutung von Trauer-Regulationsstrategien

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/1616-3443/a000548

Zusammenfassung.Theoretischer Hintergrund: Ergebnisse aus Querschnittstudien weisen darauf hin, dass Emotionsregulationsstrategien den Effekt zwischen dem Emotionsbewusstsein und depressiven Symptomen vermitteln. Es fehlen jedoch prospektive Studien, die diesen Effekt bestätigen. Fragestellung: Diese Studie überprüft, ob die Veränderung in der Nutzung adaptiver und maladaptiver Trauer-Regulationsstrategien über drei Monate die Beziehung zwischen dem Emotionsbewusstsein zu T1 und der depressiven Symptomatik zu T2 vermittelt. Methodik: Bei N = 136 Jugendlichen (55.9 % ♀, M = 11.53 Jahre; SD = 0.82) wurde zu T1 das Emotionsbewusstsein erfasst. Adaptive sowie maladaptive Strategien zur Trauerregulation und die Depressionssymptomatik wurden zu zwei Messzeitpunkten, zwischen denen ein Abstand von M = 15 Wochen (SD = 4.22) lag, erhoben. Ergebnisse: Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass ein hohes Emotionsbewusstsein zu T1 über eine Zunahme maladaptiver Regulationsstrategien mit einer stärkeren depressiven Symptomatik zu T2 verknüpft ist. Ein vermittelnder Effekt über die Veränderung adaptiver Regulationsstrategien fand sich nicht. Eine Zunahme adaptiver Strategien stand in Zusammenhang mit niedrigen Depressionswerten zu T2. Schlussfolgerung: In der Prävention sollte das Emotionsbewusstsein in Kombination mit Techniken zur Beendigung maladaptiver Trauer-Regulationsstrategien vermittelt werden. Adaptive Strategien zur Bewältigung von Trauer sollten systematisch aufgebaut und eingeübt werden.


Emotional Awareness and Depressive Symptoms in Early Adolescence. About the Meaning of Strategies for the Regulation of Sadness

Abstract. Background: Results from cross-sectional studies indicate that emotion regulation strategies mediate the effect of emotional awareness on depressive symptoms. Prospective studies, that confirm this effect are missing. Objective: The current study investigates, if changes in use of adaptive and maladaptive strategies for the regulation of sadness over three months mediate the relationship between emotional awareness at T1 and depressive symptoms at T2. Methods: In a sample of N = 136 adolescents (55.9 % ♀, M = 11.53 years; SD = 0.82) emotional awareness was self-reported. Adaptive and maladaptive strategies for the regulation of sadness and depressive symptoms were assessed at two measuring points with an interval of 15 weeks (SD = 4.22). Results: Results show, that high emotional awareness at T1 was associated with more depressive symptoms at T2 via an increase in maladaptive strategies. No mediating effect was found for changes in adaptive strategies. An increase in adaptive strategies was associated with low depressive symptoms at T2. Conclusions: In prevention, emotional awareness should be taught in combination with techniques for stopping maladaptive regulation strategies of sadness. Adaptive strategies for the regulation of sadness should be systematically build and practiced.

Literatur

  • Ahmed, S. P., Bittencourt-Hewitt, A. & Sebastian, C. L. (2015). Neurocognitive bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 11 – 25. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barnow, S., Aldinger, M., Ulrich, I. & Stopsack, M. (2013). Emotionsregulation bei Depression. Ein multimethodaler Überblick. Psychologische Rundschau, 64, 235 – 243. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Belden, A. C., Luby, J. L., Pagliaccio, D. & Barch, D. M. (2014). Neural activation associated with the cognitive emotion regulation of sadness in healthy children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 136 – 147. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beck, A. T. & Bredemeier, K. (2016). A unified model of depression: Integrating clinical, cognitive, biological and evolutionary perspectives. Clinical Psychological Science, 4, 596 – 619. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Boden, M. T. & Thompson, R. J. (2015). Facets of emotional awareness and associations with emotion regulation and depression. Emotion, 15, 399 – 410. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costello, E. J., Copleand, W. & Angold, A. (2011). Trends in psychopathology across the adolescent years: What changes when children become adolescents, and when adolescents become adults? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 1015 – 1025. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eastabrook, J. M., Flynn, J. J. & Hollenstein, T. (2014). Internalizing symptoms in female adolescents: Associations with emotional awareness and emotion regulation. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 487 – 496. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eccles, J. S. & Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as developmental contexts across adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 225 – 241. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garnefski, N. & Kraaij, N. (2006). Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: A comparative study of five specific samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1659 – 1669. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garnefski, N. & Kraaij, V. (2018). Specificity of relations between adolescents’ cognitive emotion regulation strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 1401 – 1408. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grob, A. & Smolenski, C. (2005). Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen (FEEL-KJ). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. & Jazaieri, H. (2014). , Emotion, emotion regulation and psychopathology: An affective science perspective. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 487 – 401. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Joormann, J. & Stanton, C. H. (2016). Examining emotion regulation in depression: A review and future directions. Behavior Therapy and Research, 86, 35 – 49. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karnaze, M. M. & Levine, L. (2018). : Sadness, the architect of cognitive change when and why emotions help us. In H. Lench (Ed.), The function of emotions (p. 45 – 58). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F. & McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation: Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 10 – 16. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kovacs, M., Rottenberg, J. & George, C. (2009). Maladaptive mood repair responses distinguish young adults with early-onset depressive disorders and predict future depression outcomes. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1841 – 1854. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuppens, P. & Verduyn, P. (2015). Looking at emotion regulation through the window of emotion dynamics. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 72 – 79. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kranzler, A., Young, J. F., Hankin, B. L., Abela, J. R. Z., Elias, M. J. & Selby, E. A. (2016). Emotional awareness: A transdiagnostic predictor of depression and anxiety for children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 262 – 269. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lange, S. & Tröster, H. (2018). Emotionsregulation von Jugendlichen mit depressiven Störungen. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 67, 549 – 567. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lennarz, H. K., Hollenstein, T., Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A., Knutsche, E. & Granic, I. (2019). Emotion regulation in action: Use, selection, and success of emotion regulation in adolescents’ daily lives. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43, 1 – 11. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maciejewski, D. F., van Lier, P. A. C., Branje, S. J. T., Meeus, W. H. J. & Koot, H. M. (2015). A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Development, 86, 1908 – 1921. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mullarkey, M. C., Marchetti, I. & Beevers, C. G. (2019). Using network analysis to identify central symptoms of adolescent depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48, 656 – 668. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nitkowski, D., Fern, J., Petermann, U., Petermann, F. & Zeman, J. (2019). Factorial structure of the German version of the Emotion Expression Scale for Children in early adolescents. Journal of Psychoeductional Assessment, 37, 517 – 523. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Penza-Clyve, S. & Zeman, J. (2002). Initial validation of the Emotion Expression Scale for Children (EESC). Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 31, 540 – 547. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F., Jascenoka, J., Koglin, U., Karpinski, N. & Kullik., A. (2012). Zur Lebenszeitprävalenz Affektiver Störungen im Jugendalter, Kindheit und Entwicklung, 21, 209 – 218. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Rieffe, C. & De Rooij, M. (2012). The longitudinal relationship between emotion awareness and internalising symptoms during late childhood. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 21, 349 – 356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rieffe, C., Oosterveld, P., Miers, A. C., Meerum, Terwogt, M., M. & Ly, V. (2008). Emotion awareness and internalising symptoms in children and adolescents: The Emotion Awareness Questionnaire revised. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 756 – 761. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rueth, J.-L., Lohaus, A. & Vierhaus, M. (2019). The German version of the emotion Awareness Questionnaire for children and adolescents: Associations with emotion regulation and psychosocial adjustment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 101, 434 – 445. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schäfer, J. Ö., Naumann, E., Holmes, E. A., Tuschen-Caffier, B. & Samson, A. C. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 261 – 276. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmitt, M. & Maes, J. (2000). Vorschlag zur Vereinfachung des Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI). Diagnostica, 46, 38 – 46. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Sendzik, L., Schäfer, J. Ö., Samson, A. C., Naumann, E. & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2017). Emotional awareness in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 687 – 700. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L. & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74, 1869 – 1180. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tabachnik, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2014). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Harlow, UK: Pearson Education Limited. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Van Beveren, M.-L., Goossens, L., Volkaert, B., Grassmann, C., Wante, L., Vandegeweghe, L. et al. (2019). How do I feel right now? Emotional awareness, emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in youth. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 389 – 398. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wante, L., Van Beveren, M.-L., Theuwis, L. & Braet, C. (2018). The effects of emotion regulation strategies on positive and negative affect in early adolescents. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 988 – 1002. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. & Skinner, E. (2011). The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative research and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 1 – 17. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar