Skip to main content
Studie

Kindliche Selbst- und elterliche Ko-Regulation parallel erfassen

Der IMMA 1 – 6 (IMpuls-MAnagement) Fragebogen für 1- bis 6-Jährige

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000270

Zusammenfassung. Im Umgang mit eigenen Impulsen sind Kinder zunächst auf ihre Bezugspersonen angewiesen, lernen aber schon bald, sich auch selbst zu regulieren. Im Artikel wird diskutiert, welche Faktoren dieses Entwicklungsgeschehen beeinflussen. Dabei ist es wichtig, das Verhalten von Eltern und Kindern parallel in den Blick zu nehmen. Wir stellen einen neuen Fragebogen zur Erfassung des IMpuls-MAnagements zwischen Bezugsperson und Kind vor, der für 1- bis 6-Jährige konzipiert wurde (IMMA 1 – 6). Hier macht die Bezugsperson Angaben über ihre Vorstellungen und Ziele zur kindlichen Selbstregulation (n = 16 Items), das selbstregulative Verhalten des Kindes (n = 42 Items) sowie das eigene Erziehungsverhalten (n = 55 Items) beim kindlichen Umgang mit Ziel-Frustration, Verboten und Erwartungen. Eine testtheoretische Überprüfung des Instrumentes an N = 270 Eltern-Kind Dyaden lieferte ermutigende Befunde: Sie dokumentiert gute Skalen- bzw. Item-Kennwerte. Implikationen für Forschung und Praxis werden diskutiert.


Parallel Assessment of Children’s Self-Regulation and Caregivers’ Coregulation: The IMMA (IMpulse-MAnagement) Questionnaire for
1 – 6-Year-Olds

Abstract. Whereas newborns largely depend upon their caregivers when trying to regulate their inner states, parental coregulation gradually decreases until school age. Existing work reveals that social-interactive processes influence this development, thus pointing to the need for a better understanding of the interplay between parental coregulation and children’s self-regulation throughout early childhood. Diagnostic instruments assessing the behavior of both parties in everyday situations are still missing, however. IMMA (IMpulse-MAnagement) 1 – 6, a new questionnaire to measure impulse management in the caregiver–child dyad (child age: 1 – 6 years) provides an exception. This inventory consists of three main parts: In Part 1, caregivers take the age of the given child as reference point for evaluating beliefs and goals regarding self-regulation skills when children (a) fail to achieve a self-set goal, (b) need to respond to external demands, and (c) face prohibitions. In Part 2, caregivers describe typical responses of the child handling the aforementioned situations (a–c). In Part 3, caregivers report their own responses to the child’s behavior. Using data from 270 caregiver–child dyads, we tested the psychometric quality of IMMA 1 – 6. Findings largely replicated the factor structure documented for the original IMMA 1 – 6 version: For Part 1 (n = 16 items), we identified three main factors: caregivers’ beliefs and goals regarding children’s responses when dealing with situations involving (a) goal frustration, (b) external demands, and (c) external prohibitions. For Part 2 (n = 42 items), we found eight factors: Referring to situations of type (a), we found (1) endurance, and (2) expression of negative emotions / aggression. Six factors refer to situations of type (b) and/or (c) describing the child’s response to external demand and prohibitions as follows: (1) immediate acceptance, (2) attempts to negotiate, (3) obedience following verbal pressure to meet external demands and (4) to respect prohibitions, (5) evasiveness/ignorance, (6) and obedience in the light of physical pressure. For Part 3 (n = 55 items), we identified seven factors: Two involve caregivers’ use of positive reinforcement: (1) rewarding, and (2) praising. Two factors describe attempts to consider the child’s will: (3) giving in and (4) negotiating/discussing. Another two factors target attempts to change of the child’s mental state: (5) distraction and (6) call for self-regulation; one factor aims at the forced implementation of the caregivers’ will: (7) negative pressure. All scales reveal very good internal consistencies and item characteristics as well as plausible inter-correlations. In sum, IMMA 1 – 6 provides a promising new instrument for assess coregulation and self-regulation in the caregiver–child dyad. Its potential use for research as well as for practical applications is discussed.

Literatur

  • Bechtel-Kuehne, S., Strodthoff, C. A. & Pauen, S. (2016). Co- and self-regulation in the caregiver-child dyad: Parental expectations, children’s compliance, and parental practices during early years. Journal of Self-Regulation and Regulation, 2, 32 – 56. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M. & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child evelopment, 81, 326 – 339. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H. & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 327 – 336. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cuevas, K., Deater-Deckard, K., Kim-Spoon, J., Watson, A. J., Morasch, K. C. & Bell, M. A. (2014). What’s mom got to do with it? Contributions of maternal executive function and caregiving to the development of executive function across early childhood. Developmental Science, 17, 224 – 238. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Daseking, M. & Petermann, F. (2013). Verhaltensinventar zur Beurteilung exekutiver Funktionen für das Kindergartenalter (BRIEF-P). Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) von G. A. Gioia, K. Andrews Espy und P. K. Isquith. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. & Spinrad, T. L. (2011). Effortful control – Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In K. Vohs R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 263 – 283). New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J. & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 258 – 264. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. Chicago (IL): Chicago University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C. & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172 – 179. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E. & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 1, 31 – 60. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garon, N., Smith, I. M. & Bryson, S. E. (2014). A novel executive function battery for preschoolers: Sensitivity to age differences. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 713 – 736. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gartstein, M. A. & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 64 – 86. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gioia, G. A., Espy, K. A. & Isquith, P. K. (2003). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grob, A. & Smolenski, C. (2005). Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen (FEEL-KJ). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gunzenhauser, C., Fäsche, A., Friedlmeier, W. & Suchodoletz, A. von (2014). Face it or hide it: parental socialization of reappraisal and response suppression. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1 – 14. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hammond, S. I., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I. M., Bibok, M. B. & Liebermann-Finestone, D. P. (2012). The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers’ executive function. Developmental Psychology, 48, 271 – 281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holodynski, M. & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Emotionen: Entwicklung und Regulation. Heidelberg: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Johansson, M., Marciszko, C., Brocki, K. & Bohlin, G. (2015). Individual differences in early executive functions: A longitudinal study from 12 to 36 months. Infant and Child Development, 25, 533 – 549. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 141 – 151. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karreman, A., Tuijl, C. van, Aken, M. A. G. van & Dekovic, M. (2008). Parenting, coparenting, and effortful control in preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 30 – 40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kiss, M., Fechete, G., Pop, M. & Susa, G. (2014). Early childhood self-regulation in context: Parental and familial environmental influences. Cognition, Brain, Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 55 – 85. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Koglin, U. & Petermann, F. (2016). Verhaltensskalen für das Kindergartenalter (VSK). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kullik, A. & Petermann, F. (2011). Zum gegenwärtigen Stand der Emotionsregulationsdiagnostik im Säuglings- und Kleinkindalter. Diagnostica, 57, 165 – 178. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Laukkanen, J., Ojansuu, U., Tolvanen, A., Alatupa, S. & Aunola, K. (2014). Child’s difficult temperament and mothers’ parenting styles. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 312 – 323. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J. & Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 2693 – 2698. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S. & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361 – 388. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A.H., Howeter, A. & Wagner, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe“ tasks: A latent variable analysis, Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49 – 100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Naumann, S., Bertram, H., Kuschel, A., Heinrichs, N., Hahlweg, K. & Döpfner, M. (2010). Der Erziehungsfragebogen (EFB). Diagnostica, 56, 144 – 157. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Neuenschwander, R., Röthlisberger, M., Cimeli, P. & Roebers, C. M. (2012). How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 353 – 371. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nigg, J. T. (2017). On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 361 – 383. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Otterpohl, N., Imort, S., Lohaus, A. & Heinrichs, N. (2012). Kindliche Regulation von Wut: Effekte familiärer Kontextfaktoren. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 21, 47 – 56. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Papoušek, M., Schieche, M. & Wurmser, H (2004). Regulationsstörungen der frühen Kindheit. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pauen, S., EDOS Group (2016). Understanding early development of self-regulation and co-regulation: EDOS and PROSECO. Journal of Self-Regulation and Regulation, 2, 2 – 17. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pauen, S., Hochmuth, A., Schulz, A. & Bechtel, S. (2014). IMMA 1 – 6: IMpuls-MAnagement vom Kleinkind- bis zum Vorschulalter – Ein Elternfragebogen zur Beziehungsgestaltung im Umgang mit Erwartungen, Zielen und Gefühlen. Kindergartenpädagogik – Online-Handbuch. http://www.kindergartenpaedagogik.de/2308.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., Dollar, J. M., Keane, S. P. & Shanahan, L. (2018). Self-regulation as a predictor of patterns of change in externalizing behaviors from infancy to adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 497 – 510. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A. & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 386 – 401. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richland, L. E. & Burchinal, M. R. (2013). Early executive function predicts reasoning development. Psychological Science, 24, 87 – 92. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L. & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394 – 1408. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothbart, M. K. & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. DamonR. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99 – 166). New York: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sanders, M. R. & Mazzuchelli, T. G. (2018). The power of positive parenting. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., Deković, M. & Aken, M. A. G. van (2016). Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 1068 – 1110. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Song, J.-H., Miller, A. L., Leung, C. Y. Y., Lumeng, J. C. & Rosenblum, K. L. (2018). Positive Parenting Moderates the Association between Temperament and Self-Regulation in Low-Income Toddlers. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27, 2354 – 2364. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, A. & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ulrich, F. & Petermann, F. (2017). Elterliche Emotionsdysregulation als Risikofaktor für die Kindliche Entwicklung. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 133 – 146. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Valcan, D. S., Davis, H. & Pino-Pasternak, D. (2017). Parental behaviours predicting early childhood executive functions: a meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 607 – 649. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Haugen, R. & Marilyn, S. (2013). Effortful control and impulsivity as concurrent and longitudinal predictors of academic achievement. Journal of Early Adolescence, 33, 946 – 972. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vonderlin, E., Ropeter, A. & Pauen, S. (2012). Erfassung des frühkindlichen Temperaments mit dem Infant Behavior Questionnaire Revised – Psychometrische Merkmale einer deutschen Version. Zeitschrift Für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 40, 307 – 314. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar